全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1353篇 |
免费 | 17篇 |
专业分类
1370篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 5篇 |
2023年 | 13篇 |
2022年 | 8篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 28篇 |
2019年 | 29篇 |
2018年 | 32篇 |
2017年 | 46篇 |
2016年 | 48篇 |
2015年 | 46篇 |
2014年 | 55篇 |
2013年 | 174篇 |
2012年 | 79篇 |
2011年 | 85篇 |
2010年 | 57篇 |
2009年 | 51篇 |
2008年 | 66篇 |
2007年 | 86篇 |
2006年 | 59篇 |
2005年 | 52篇 |
2004年 | 52篇 |
2003年 | 33篇 |
2002年 | 35篇 |
2001年 | 19篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 23篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 14篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 8篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 10篇 |
1991年 | 6篇 |
1990年 | 8篇 |
1989年 | 9篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 4篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 4篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1973年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1964年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有1370条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
931.
Rhodes G Lee K Palermo R Weiss M Yoshikawa S Clissa P Williams T Peters M Winkler C Jeffery L 《Perception》2005,34(3):319-340
Averaged face composites, which represent the central tendency of a familiar population of faces, are attractive. If this prototypicality contributes to their appeal, then averaged composites should be more attractive when their component faces come from a familiar, own-race population than when they come from a less familiar, other-race population. We compared the attractiveness of own-race composites, other-race composites, and mixed-race composites (where the component faces were from both races). In experiment 1, Caucasian participants rated own-race composites as more attractive than other-race composites, but only for male faces. However, mixed-race (Caucasian/Japanese) composites were significantly more attractive than own-race composites, particularly for the opposite sex. In experiment 2, Caucasian and Japanese participants living in Australia and Japan, respectively, selected the most attractive face from a continuum with exaggerated Caucasian characteristics at one end and exaggerated Japanese characteristics at the other, with intervening images including a Caucasian averaged composite, a mixed-race averaged composite, and a Japanese averaged composite. The most attractive face was, again, a mixed-race composite, for both Caucasian and Japanese participants. In experiment 3, Caucasian participants rated individual Eurasian faces as significantly more attractive than either Caucasian or Asian faces. Similar results were obtained with composites. Eurasian faces and composites were also rated as healthier than Caucasian or Asian faces and composites, respectively. These results suggest that signs of health may be more important than prototypicality in making average faces attractive. 相似文献
932.
It is widely assumed that emotionally avoidant or defensive individuals will have a difficult time adjusting to the loss of a loved one. However, recent research suggests that defensive individuals tend to adapt quite well to loss. Such findings pose a number of challenges to attachment theory--a theory that has traditionally held that emotional avoidance is indicative of poor psychological adjustment. In this article, the authors argue that contemporary models of individual differences in adult attachment allow the derivation of at least three competing hypotheses regarding the relationship between avoidant attachment and adaptation to loss. These hypotheses are tested using two-wave data on 59 bereaved adults. Results indicate that whereas some avoidant individuals (i.e., those who are fearfully avoidant) have a difficult time adapting to the loss of a loved one, other avoidant adults (i.e., those who are dismissingly avoidant) show a pattern of resilience to loss. 相似文献
933.
934.
During binocular rivalry, incompatible images presented dichoptically compete for perceptual dominance. It has long been debated whether binocular rivalry can be controlled by attention. Most studies have shown that voluntary control over binocular rivalry is limited. We sought to remove attention from binocular rivalry by presenting a concurrent task. Diverting attention slowed the rivalry alternation rate, and did so in proportion to the difficulty of the concurrent task. Even a very demanding distractor task, however, did not arrest rivalry alternations completely. Given that diverting attention was equivalent to lowering the contrast of the rival stimuli, the ability of attention to speed binocular rivalry is most likely due to an increase in the effective contrast of the stimuli through boosting the gain of the cortical response. This increase in effective contrast will ultimately lead to a perceptual switch, thereby limiting voluntary control. Thus, attention speeds rivalry alternations, but has no inherent control over the rivalry process. 相似文献
935.
We report a preliminary analysis of the interactions between eye and hand during tracing and drawing of four simple shapes. Eye and hand movements were recorded using the ASL 504 system and the Flock of Birds system, respectively. During tracing, pen tip and eye were tightly coupled, with participants making a series of small saccades just in front of the moving pen, interspersed with periods of smooth pursuit. During drawing, saccades were fewer and larger and pursuit was less frequent. Observed eye-hand interactions suggested a bidirectional relationship between the eye and hand. These findings are explained in terms of the differing degree that the two tasks employ visual detail, external or internal cues and eye-hand coordination. 相似文献
936.
937.
In this article, we describe the Naturalistic University of Alberta Nonlinear Correlation Explorer (NUANCE), a computer program
for data exploration and analysis. NUANCE is specialized for finding nonlinear relations between any number of predictors
and a dependent value to be predicted. It searches the space of possible relations between the predictors and the dependent
value by using natural selection to evolve equations that maximize the correlation between their output and the dependent
value. In this article, we introduce the program, describe how to use it, and provide illustrative examples. NUANCE is written
in Java, which runs on most computer platforms. We have contributed NUANCE to the archival Web site of the Psychonomic Society
(www.psychonomic.org/archive), from which it may be freely downloaded. 相似文献
938.
Cree GS McNorgan C McRae K 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2006,32(4):643-658
The authors present data from 2 feature verification experiments designed to determine whether distinctive features have a privileged status in the computation of word meaning. They use an attractor-based connectionist model of semantic memory to derive predictions for the experiments. Contrary to central predictions of the conceptual structure account, but consistent with their own model, the authors present empirical evidence that distinctive features of both living and nonliving things do indeed have a privileged role in the computation of word meaning. The authors explain the mechanism through which these effects are produced in their model by presenting an analysis of the weight structure developed in the network during training. 相似文献
939.
940.
In this paper we describe dissociations of implicit versus explicit access to semantic information in a patient with deep dyslexia. This acquired reading disorder is characterized by the production of morphological (e.g., SLEEP read as SLEEPING) and semantic errors (e.g., HEART read as BLOOD) and consequently provides a potential window into the operation of both aspects of the language system. The deep dyslexic patient in this study (JO) demonstrated implicit semantic access to items in a number of tasks despite the fact that she was unable to correctly read these items aloud. The findings from this study are consistent with a model of lexical deficits that distinguishes between explicit and implicit access to lexical representations on the basis of inhibitory processes. 相似文献