首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   213549篇
  免费   8998篇
  国内免费   162篇
  2020年   2818篇
  2019年   3485篇
  2018年   4134篇
  2017年   4504篇
  2016年   5040篇
  2015年   3972篇
  2014年   4831篇
  2013年   23594篇
  2012年   6111篇
  2011年   5740篇
  2010年   4614篇
  2009年   5151篇
  2008年   5686篇
  2007年   5335篇
  2006年   5385篇
  2005年   5199篇
  2004年   4729篇
  2003年   4257篇
  2002年   4093篇
  2001年   5253篇
  2000年   5013篇
  1999年   4358篇
  1998年   3046篇
  1997年   2802篇
  1996年   2697篇
  1995年   2538篇
  1994年   2511篇
  1993年   2448篇
  1992年   3588篇
  1991年   3398篇
  1990年   3252篇
  1989年   3081篇
  1988年   3048篇
  1987年   2974篇
  1986年   2989篇
  1985年   3263篇
  1984年   3033篇
  1983年   2719篇
  1982年   2550篇
  1981年   2491篇
  1980年   2343篇
  1979年   2855篇
  1978年   2534篇
  1977年   2397篇
  1976年   2250篇
  1975年   2534篇
  1974年   2701篇
  1973年   2629篇
  1972年   2155篇
  1971年   2008篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
52.
In this introduction to the special issue on teaching about gender and ethnicity in psychology, we consider the assumptions underlying an inclusive curriculum that pays attention to gender and ethnicity and address why such a curriculum has merit. We review empirical studies, assessing whether existing curricula are inclusive, and present an overview of the articles and the recurrent themes. These themes include the complexity of the interaction between ethnicity and gender; the difficulty of deciding which of the many possible ethnic groups to include in course material; the dominance of evaluative comparison in discussions of differences among groups; the interdisciplinary nature of research on ethnicity; and the tendency in psychology to ignore the importance of the power differences that confound analyses of the effect of ethnicity and gender.  相似文献   
53.
The purpose of this study is to compare field investigative interviews of children (FIIC) with three different legal outcomes in child sexual abuse cases: (i) insufficient evidence to proceed (IEP); (ii) convictions; or (iii) acquittals by the court. One hundred FIIC were divided into one of the three outcome possibilities. Amongst the female interviewees older than 10 years, there were no cases of acquittals and the convicted cases were over-represented. The children's response to open questions was found to be the main difference between the three FIIC outcomes. The responses to these open questions were 1.9 and 2.3 times longer in the convicted cases compared to acquittals and IEP. Possible explanations for the result are discussed. Copyright © 2009 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
54.
55.
56.
Adult age differences in the consistency effect were examined in 3 experiments. The consistency effect refers to items inconsistent with expectations being better remembered than items consistent with expectations. Younger and older adults walked into an office room and viewed objects that varied in their consistency with expectation. Immediate and delayed recognition tests on item information (i.e., distractors were defined by their semantic identity) revealed that both age groups recognized unexpected items better than expected items. However, when recognition of token information was requested (i.e., distractors were defined by their physical appearance), younger adults, in contrast to older adults, exhibited consistency effects. Also, under divided attention, young adults revealed the same pattern of data as did elderly adults under full attention. The results are discussed in terms of capacity-related differences in distinctive encoding.  相似文献   
57.
Range of electric vehicles (EVs) has long been considered a major barrier in acceptance of electric mobility. We examined the nature of how range is experienced in an EV and whether variables from other adaptation contexts, notably stress, have explanatory power for inter‐individual differences in what we term comfortable range. Forty EVs were leased to a sample of users for a 6‐month field study. Qualitative and quantitative analyses of range experiences were performed, including regression analyses to examine the role of stress‐buffering personality traits and coping skills in comfortable range. Users appraised range as a resource to which they could successfully adapt and that satisfied most of their daily mobility needs. However, indicators were found that suggested suboptimal range utilisation. Stress‐buffering personality traits (control beliefs, ambiguity tolerance) and coping skills (subjective range competence, daily range practice) were found to play a substantial role in comfortable range. Hence, it may be possible to overcome perceived range barriers with the assistance of psychological interventions such as information, training, and interface design. Providing drivers with a reliable usable range may be more important than enhancing maximal range in an electric mobility system.  相似文献   
58.
59.
60.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号