全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1519篇 |
免费 | 48篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
1568篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 20篇 |
2019年 | 24篇 |
2018年 | 31篇 |
2017年 | 45篇 |
2016年 | 46篇 |
2015年 | 41篇 |
2014年 | 32篇 |
2013年 | 153篇 |
2012年 | 45篇 |
2011年 | 60篇 |
2010年 | 42篇 |
2009年 | 44篇 |
2008年 | 61篇 |
2007年 | 55篇 |
2006年 | 58篇 |
2005年 | 45篇 |
2004年 | 56篇 |
2003年 | 55篇 |
2002年 | 41篇 |
2001年 | 23篇 |
2000年 | 36篇 |
1999年 | 18篇 |
1998年 | 39篇 |
1997年 | 19篇 |
1996年 | 23篇 |
1995年 | 22篇 |
1994年 | 25篇 |
1993年 | 26篇 |
1992年 | 19篇 |
1991年 | 25篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 17篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 24篇 |
1986年 | 17篇 |
1985年 | 19篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 26篇 |
1982年 | 16篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 17篇 |
1978年 | 15篇 |
1977年 | 16篇 |
1975年 | 13篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 9篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有1568条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
101.
Radomsky AS de Silva P Todd G Treasure J Murphy T 《Behaviour research and therapy》2002,40(10):1169-1177
Cognitive biases and cognitive distortions have been implicated as important factors in the development and maintenance of many disorders. The concept of thought-shape fusion (TSF) in eating disorders was developed by Shafran, Teachman, Kerry, and Rachman (British Journal of Clinical Psychology 38 (1999) 167) as a variant of thought-action fusion, described by Shafran, Thordarson and Rachman (Journal of Anxiety Disorders 10 (1996) 379). TSF occurs when thinking about eating certain types of food increases a person's estimate of their shape and/or weight, elicits a perception of moral wrongdoing, and/or makes the person feel fat. Shafran et al. (1999) examined both the psychometric and experimental properties of TSF in an undergraduate sample. This paper reports an extension of this work to a clinical group (N=20) of patients with anorexia nervosa. After completing a set of relevant questionnaires, participants were asked to think about a food which they considered extremely fattening. They were then asked to write out the sentence, "I am eating--.", inserting the name of the fattening food in the blank. After being asked to rate their anxiety, guilt, feelings about their weight, morality, etc., participants were given the opportunity to neutralize their statement in any way they chose. The majority of the participants neutralized in ways consistent with the findings of Shafran et al. (1999). The results are discussed in terms of cognitive-behavioural formulations of eating disorders, and of the influence of cognitive biases and cognitive distortions on the processing of information relevant to food, weight and shape in anorexia nervosa. 相似文献
102.
103.
Accessibility of characters in two-character sentences (e.g., The butler helped Calvin at the wedding reception) was investigated with a probe recognition task. Probes were either the first character (e.g., butler) or the second character (e.g., Calvin) in a sentence and were designated by proper names or common nouns crossed with name or noun nonprobes. Results show that (1) probes in first position are more accessible than those in second position, but not when noun probes are paired with name nonprobes, (2) characters designated by names are generally more accessible than those designated by nouns, and (3) the first name in a sentence is more available than other characters, regardless of position. Thus, accessibility of characters in a sentence seems dependent on discourse function, with named characters seen as main characters, rather than on nondiscourse-related factors, such as temporal distinctiveness. 相似文献
104.
Janet Sigal Margaret S. Gibbs Carl Goodrich Tayyab Rashid Afroze Anjum Daniel Hsu Carrol S. Perrino Hale Bolak Boratav Aggie Carson-Arenas Berna van Baarsen Joop van der Pligt Wei-Kang Pan 《Sex roles》2005,52(3-4):201-215
Male and female university students from the United States, Canada, Germany, the Netherlands, Ecuador, Pakistan, the Philippines, Taiwan, and Turkey read a standardized scenario in which a male professor was accused of sexually harassing a female graduate student. Respondents from individualist countries judged the professor to be guilty of sexual harassment more often than did those from collectivist countries. Women rendered significantly more guilty judgments and assigned more severe punishments to the accused professor than did men. Implications for the individualist–collectivist classification system and cross-cultural research are discussed. 相似文献
105.
Branigan HP Pickering MJ McLean JF 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2005,31(3):468-481
Strong evidence suggests that prior syntactic context affects language production (e.g., J. K. Bock, 1986). The authors report 4 experiments that used an expression-picture matching task to investigate whether it also affects ambiguity resolution in comprehension. All experiments examined the interpretation of prepositional phrases that were ambiguous between high and low attachment. After reading a prime expression with a high-attached interpretation, participants tended to interpret an ambiguous prepositional phrase in a target expression as highly attached if it contained the same verb as the prime (Experiment 1), but not if it contained a different verb (Experiment 2). They also tended to adopt the high-attached interpretation after producing a prime with the high-attached interpretation that included the same verb (Experiment 3). Finally, they were faster to adopt a high-attached interpretation after reading an expression containing the same verb that was disambiguated to the high-attached versus the low-attached interpretation (Experiment 4). 相似文献
106.
107.
Genetic Cancer Risk Assessment and Counseling: Recommendations of the National Society of Genetic Counselors 总被引:4,自引:0,他引:4
Trepanier A Ahrens M McKinnon W Peters J Stopfer J Grumet SC Manley S Culver JO Acton R Larsen-Haidle J Correia LA Bennett R Pettersen B Ferlita TD Costalas JW Hunt K Donlon S Skrzynia C Farrell C Callif-Daley F Vockley CW;National Society of Genetic Counselors 《Journal of genetic counseling》2004,13(2):83-114
These cancer genetic counseling recommendations describe the medical, psychosocial, and ethical ramifications of identifying at-risk individuals through cancer risk assessment with or without genetic testing. They were developed by members of the Practice Issues Subcommittee of the National Society of Genetic Counselors Cancer Genetic Counseling Special Interest Group. The information contained in this document is derived from extensive review of the current literature on cancer genetic risk assessment and counseling as well as the personal expertise of genetic counselors specializing in cancer genetics. The recommendations are intended to provide information about the process of genetic counseling and risk assessment for hereditary cancer disorders rather than specific information about individual syndromes. Key components include the intake (medical and family histories), psychosocial assessment (assessment of risk perception), cancer risk assessment (determination and communication of risk), molecular testing for hereditary cancer syndromes (regulations, informed consent, and counseling process), and follow-up considerations. These recommendations should not be construed as dictating an exclusive course of management, nor does use of such recommendations guarantee a particular outcome. These recommendations do not displace a health care provider's professional judgment based on the clinical circumstances of a client. 相似文献
108.
Do young infants treat speech as a special signal, compared with structurally similar non‐speech sounds? We presented 2‐ to 7‐month‐old infants with nonsense speech sounds and complex non‐speech analogues. The non‐speech analogues retain many of the spectral and temporal properties of the speech signal, including the pitch contour information which is known to be salient to young listeners, and thus provide a stringent test for a potential listening bias for speech. Our results show that infants as young as 2 months of age listened longer to speech sounds. This listening selectivity indicates that early‐functioning biases direct infants’ attention to speech, granting speech a special status in relation to other sounds. 相似文献
109.
Although alexithymia and psychopathy have long been linked, the relationship between the two constructs remains unclear. In this study, we used the California Q-set Alexithymia Prototype (CAQ-AP; Haviland & Reise, 1996) and Psychopathy Prototype (CAQ-PP; Reise & Oliver, 1994) to clarify the relationship between the two constructs and evaluate both in a sample of contemporary and historical political leaders (N = 42). Our data show that both individuals with prototypic alexithymia and psychopathy lack empathy and insight and are not introspective. The prototypic person with alexithymia, however, is anxious, overcontrolled, submissive, boring, ethically consistent, and socially conforming, whereas the prototypic individual with psychopathy is anxiety-free, undercontrolled, dominant, charming, deceitful, and nonconforming. Characteristics of both were relatively common among the 13 controversial and notorious leaders and relatively uncommon among the 29 generally respected leaders in the sample. The CAQ-AP and the CAQ-PP appear to be useful for evaluating alexithymic and psychopathic features in public figures. 相似文献
110.