首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   171287篇
  免费   7684篇
  国内免费   157篇
  2020年   2794篇
  2019年   3441篇
  2018年   3436篇
  2017年   3913篇
  2016年   4578篇
  2015年   3935篇
  2014年   4801篇
  2013年   23504篇
  2012年   4424篇
  2011年   3446篇
  2010年   3795篇
  2009年   4732篇
  2008年   3692篇
  2007年   3198篇
  2006年   3854篇
  2005年   3880篇
  2004年   3386篇
  2003年   3046篇
  2002年   2831篇
  2001年   3079篇
  2000年   2919篇
  1999年   2965篇
  1998年   2807篇
  1997年   2649篇
  1996年   2568篇
  1995年   2394篇
  1994年   2367篇
  1993年   2320篇
  1992年   2505篇
  1991年   2363篇
  1990年   2202篇
  1989年   2161篇
  1988年   2102篇
  1987年   2167篇
  1986年   2151篇
  1985年   2381篇
  1984年   2498篇
  1983年   2269篇
  1982年   2365篇
  1981年   2337篇
  1980年   2159篇
  1979年   2191篇
  1978年   2146篇
  1977年   2148篇
  1976年   1926篇
  1975年   2015篇
  1974年   2054篇
  1973年   1957篇
  1972年   1553篇
  1971年   1513篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
171.
A series of four spatial localization experiments is reported that examined the effects of display duration and presentation mode on positive and negative priming using an attended-repetition and an ignored-repetition paradigm, respectively. Experiment 1 showed larger positive priming with response-dependent than with 150 ms display durations while negative priming remained unaffected. Experiments 2-4 were performed to further elucidate the effects of prime-probe durations. Data suggest largely independent effects of prime and probe duration on priming effects. Manipulation of prime duration affected facilitation due to repetition of the prime distractor location as well as inhibitory effects associated with ignored repetition. Furthermore, anticipated probe duration modulated the effectiveness of inhibition of return. Findings are discussed within a framework proposing two major components of priming effects—a stimulus-driven or automatic component, and a strategic component related to the participant's expectations towards the probe.  相似文献   
172.
In this study, we explored the structure of pupils’ creativity in primary education following the Amusement Park Theory, by investigating undiscovered linkages between the domains of writing, mathematics, and drawing. More specifically, we examined: (a) whether some domains and general thematic areas are more closely related to each other than to others, (b) whether literacy and mathematical ability are specific underlying traits of creativity in writing and mathematics, respectively, and (c) whether intelligence and divergent thinking are related to creativity in all domains. The sample consisted of 331 Dutch 4th grade pupils. For each research question, a model was analyzed using structural equation modeling. We found creativity in mathematics and creativity in writing to be most similar, followed by creativity in mathematics and creativity in drawing, with creativity in writing and creativity in drawing being least similar. Additionally, we found evidence for several underlying traits (i.e., literacy ability and mathematical ability) and initial requirements of creativity (i.e., intelligence and divergent thinking), none of which were important for creativity in only one domain, and of which only intelligence was important for creativity in all domains. Herewith, our study provides insights regarding the complexity of the structure of creativity in primary education.  相似文献   
173.
174.
175.
176.
177.
178.
Two experiments examined repetition priming in the recognition of famous voices. In Experiment 1, reaction times for fame decisions to famous voice samples were shorter than in an unprimed condition, when voices were primed by a different voice sample of the same person having been presented in an earlier phase of the experiment. No effect of voice repetition was observed for non-famous voices. In Experiment 2, it was investigated whether this priming effect is voice-specific or whether it is related to post-perceptual processes in person recognition. Recognizing a famous voice was again primed by having earlier heard a different voice sample of that person. Although an earlier exposure to that person's name did not cause any priming, there was some indication of priming following an earlier exposure to that person's face. Finally, earlier exposure to the identical voice sample (as compared to a different voice sample from the same person) caused a considerable bias towards responding 'famous'-i.e. performance benefits for famous but costs for nonfamous voices. The findings suggestthat (1) repetition priming invoice recognition primarily involves the activation of perceptual representations of voices, and (2) it is important to determine the conditions in which priming causes bias effects that need to be disentangled from performance benefits.  相似文献   
179.
180.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号