全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2449篇 |
免费 | 37篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
2487篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 13篇 |
2023年 | 18篇 |
2022年 | 22篇 |
2021年 | 40篇 |
2020年 | 40篇 |
2019年 | 59篇 |
2018年 | 74篇 |
2017年 | 78篇 |
2016年 | 94篇 |
2015年 | 59篇 |
2014年 | 77篇 |
2013年 | 305篇 |
2012年 | 136篇 |
2011年 | 114篇 |
2010年 | 104篇 |
2009年 | 77篇 |
2008年 | 108篇 |
2007年 | 100篇 |
2006年 | 94篇 |
2005年 | 82篇 |
2004年 | 77篇 |
2003年 | 80篇 |
2002年 | 66篇 |
2001年 | 42篇 |
2000年 | 37篇 |
1999年 | 37篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 30篇 |
1995年 | 20篇 |
1994年 | 15篇 |
1993年 | 13篇 |
1992年 | 28篇 |
1991年 | 19篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 11篇 |
1988年 | 23篇 |
1987年 | 17篇 |
1986年 | 20篇 |
1985年 | 12篇 |
1984年 | 25篇 |
1982年 | 9篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1978年 | 10篇 |
1973年 | 8篇 |
1972年 | 13篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1969年 | 11篇 |
1968年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有2487条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
911.
Semisupervised category learning: The impact of feedback in learning the information-integration task 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Katleen Vandist Maarten De Schryver Yves Rosseel 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2009,71(2):328-341
In a standard supervised classification paradigm, stimuli are presented sequentially, participants make a classification, and feedback follows immediately. In this article, we use a semisupervised classification paradigm, in which feedback is given after a prespecified percentage of trials only. In Experiment 1, feedback was given in 100%, 0%, 25%, and 50% of the trials. Previous research reported by Ashby, Queller, and Berretty (1999) indicated that in an information-integration task, perfect accuracy was obtained supervised (100%) but not unsupervised (0%). Our results show that in both the 100% and 50% conditions, participants were able to achieve maximum accuracy. However, in the 0% and the 25% conditions, participants failed to learn. To discover the influence of the no-feedback trials on the learning process, the 50% condition was replicated in Experiment 2, substituting unrelated filler trials for the no-feedback trials. The results indicated that accuracy rates were similar, suggesting no impact of the no-feedback trials on the learning process. The possibility of ever learning in a 25% setting was also researched in Experiment 2. Using twice as many trials, the results showed that all but 2 participants succeeded, suggesting that only the total number of feedback trials is important. The impact of the semisupervised learning results for ALCOVE, COVIS, and SPEED models is discussed. 相似文献
912.
An increasing number of studies have demonstrated that a response in one task can be activated automatically on the basis merely of instructed stimulus–response (S–R) mappings belonging to another task. Such instruction-based response activations are considered to be evidence for the formation of S–R associations on the basis of the S–R mappings for an upcoming, but not yet executed, task. A crucial but somewhat neglected assumption is that instructed S–R associations are formed only under conditions that impose a sufficient degree of task preparation. Accordingly, in the present study we investigated the relation between task preparation and the instruction-based task-rule congruency effect, which is an index of response activation on the basis of instructions. The results from two experiments demonstrated that merely instructed S–R mappings of a particular task only elicit instruction-based response activations when that task is prepared for to a sufficient degree. Implications are discussed for the representation of instructed S–R mappings in working memory. 相似文献
913.
Paul R. J. Falger Erik G. W. Schouten Ad W. P. M. Appels Yolande C. M. De Vos 《Psychology & health》2013,28(3):231-258
Abstract Prodromal symptoms of vital exhaustion and sleep complaints are increasingly being recognized as important risk indicators for myocardial infarction (MI). In this study, the relationships between vital exhaustion, behavioral characteristics, and MI, and between several sleep complaints and MI were investigated. The subjects were 133 male cases with first MI, 192 male hospital controls, and 133 male neighborhood controls. The instruments used were the Maastricht Questionnaire (vital exhaustion), the Life Styles Questionnaire (sleep complaints, coffee consumption, and current smoking), and the Structured Interview (Type A behavior). All sleep complaints, vital exhaustion, Type A behavior, excessive coffee consumption, and current smoking occurred significantly more often in MI-cases. After controlling for these potential confounders, and age, most sleep complaints retained significant associations with MI, in particular often taking naps during the day and waking up in the morning feeling tired and exhausted. 相似文献
914.
Coping with dental treatment correlates of dispositional and domain specific monitoring and blunting
Florence J. Van Zuuren Ad De Jongh Carin Beekers Petrouschka Swinkels 《Psychology & health》2013,28(2):323-337
Abstract Relations between coping style and several variables on an intermediary level were studied in a group of dentally anxious patients (N = 68) awaiting dental treatment. Monitoring was found to be positively related to the need of information: General dispositional monitoring was the best predictor of the need of information in case of more distant threat, whereas domain specific monitoring was the best predictor in case of imminent threat. Furthermore. dental trait anxiety was strongly related to early anticipatory tension, degree of anticipatory tension at several moments in time, and tension during treatment as perceived by the dentist, thereby overshadowing the weaker effects of coping style. Domain specific blunting was negatively related to desires for informational adaptations in the waiting room, and to question proneness as perceived by the dentist during treatment. It is concluded that, in order to study further effects of coping style, a sample homogeneous with respect to domain specific anxiety is needed. 相似文献
915.
The flow of liquid through foam confined in vertical tubes was investigated by measuring the velocity vf of the liquid front forced down by gravity for various flow rates Q. The power law relating the velocity to flow rate of the incoming liquid (vf 相似文献
916.
The goal of this research is to test the hypothesis that a category is not necessarily represented by all observed exemplars,
but by a reduced subset of these exemplars. To test this hypothesis, we made use of a study reported by Nosofsky, Clark, and
Shin (1989), and replicated their Experiment 1 in order to gather individual-participant data. Both a full exemplar model
and a reduced exemplar model were fit to the data. In general, the fits of the reduced exemplar model were superior to those
of the full exemplar model. The results suggest that only a subset of exemplars may be sufficient for category representation. 相似文献
917.
Current theories of social cognition are mainly based on a representationalist view. Moreover, they focus on a rather sophisticated
and limited aspect of understanding others, i.e. on how we predict and explain others’ behaviours through representing their
mental states. Research into the ‘social brain’ has also favoured a third-person paradigm of social cognition as a passive
observation of others’ behaviour, attributing it to an inferential, simulative or projective process in the individual brain.
In this paper, we present a concept of social understanding as an ongoing, dynamical process of participatory sense-making
and mutual incorporation. This process may be described (1) from a dynamical agentive systems point of view as an interaction and coordination of two embodied agents; (2) from a phenomenological approach as a mutual incorporation, i.e. a process in which the lived bodies of both participants extend and form a common intercorporality. Intersubjectivity,
it is argued, is not a solitary task of deciphering or simulating the movements of others but means entering a process of
embodied interaction and generating common meaning through it. This approach will be further illustrated by an analysis of
primary dyadic interaction in early childhood. 相似文献
918.
Anja Van den Broeck Bert Schreurs Hans De Witte Maarten Vansteenkiste Filip Germeys Wilmar Schaufeli 《Psychologie appliquee》2011,60(4):600-621
In order to explain the diverging well‐being outcomes of workaholism, this study aimed to examine the motivational orientations that may fuel the two main components of workaholism (i.e. working excessively and working compulsively). Drawings on Self‐Determination Theory, both autonomous and controlled motivation were suggested to drive excessive work, which therefore was expected to relate positively to both well‐being (i.e. vigor) and ill‐health (i.e. exhaustion). Compulsive work, in contrast, was hypothesised to originate exclusively out of controlled motivation and therefore to only associate positively with ill‐being. Structural equation modeling in a heterogeneous sample of Belgian white‐collar workers (N= 370) confirmed that autonomous motivation associated positively with excessive work, which then related positively to vigor. Controlled motivation correlated positively with compulsive work, which therefore related positively with exhaustion. The hypothesised path from controlled motivation to exhaustion through excessive work was not corroborated. In general, the findings suggest that primarily compulsive work yields associations with ill‐being, since it may stem from a qualitatively inferior type of motivation. 相似文献
919.
Apraxic agraphia is a writing disorder due to a loss or lack of access to motor engrams that program the movements necessary to produce letters. Clinical and functional neuroimaging studies have demonstrated that the neural network responsible for writing includes the superior parietal region and the dorsolateral and medial premotor cortex. Recent studies of two cases with atypical lesion localisations in the left thalamus and the right cerebellum support the hypothesis that the written language network is larger than previously assumed. The aim of this study is twofold: (1) to provide a survey of cases of apraxic agraphia published between 1973 and June 2010, and (2) to provide further evidence for a role of the cerebellum in writing via three additional cases who presented with apraxic agraphia after ischemic damage in the cerebellum. Functional neuroimaging studies by means of brain perfusion SPECT showed perfusional deficits in the anatomoclinically suspected supratentorial areas, subserving language dynamics, syntax, naming, writing and executive functioning. 相似文献
920.
Shaul Shalvi Jason Dana Michel J.J. Handgraaf Carsten K.W. De Dreu 《Organizational behavior and human decision processes》2011
Employing a die-under-cup paradigm, we study the extent to which people lie when it is transparently clear they cannot be caught. We asked participants to report the outcome of a private die roll and gain money according to their reports. Results suggest that the degree of lying depends on the extent to which self-justifications are available. Specifically, when people are allowed to roll the die three times to ensure its legitimacy, but only the first roll is supposed to “count,” we find evidence that the highest outcome of the three rolls is reported. Eliminating the ability to observe more than one roll reduces lying. Additional results suggest that observing desired counterfactuals, in the form of additional rolls not meant to determine pay, attenuates the degree to which people perceive lies as unethical. People seem to derive value from self-justifications allowing them to lie for money while feeling honest. 相似文献