全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2107篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
2134篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 12篇 |
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 25篇 |
2020年 | 35篇 |
2019年 | 43篇 |
2018年 | 63篇 |
2017年 | 74篇 |
2016年 | 74篇 |
2015年 | 55篇 |
2014年 | 57篇 |
2013年 | 261篇 |
2012年 | 118篇 |
2011年 | 110篇 |
2010年 | 95篇 |
2009年 | 60篇 |
2008年 | 89篇 |
2007年 | 110篇 |
2006年 | 81篇 |
2005年 | 68篇 |
2004年 | 71篇 |
2003年 | 58篇 |
2002年 | 54篇 |
2001年 | 32篇 |
2000年 | 29篇 |
1999年 | 17篇 |
1998年 | 22篇 |
1997年 | 25篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 17篇 |
1993年 | 21篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 19篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1966年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有2134条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
331.
332.
Jan Erik Christensen 《亚洲哲学》2014,24(3):279-293
From the ‘DuPont factory’ case in China, one can see that contemporary ecology is faced with two underlying problems: A lack of responsibility toward the environment beyond being economically profitable, and a lack of care for what is outside of one’s immediate environment. For the purpose of confronting these two problems, I suggest two Confucian concepts: 1. zhengming 正名 (Rectification of names) and 2. datong 大同 (Great Harmony). These two concepts can be used to develop an environmental ethics and thus play a crucial role in solving the two above-mentioned problems. Zhengming is originally a political concept that was put forward by Confucius. According to this concept, everyone should fulfill their duty to the utmost in accordance with their individual roles. Datong is Confucius’ political ideal and emphasizes gong 公 (public) as a spirit for caring for tian xia 天下 (all under Heaven). A Confucian-style harmonious society is achieved by ‘letting every being manifest its mandate to the full’ (ge jin xing fen 各盡性分). When we turn toward the natural environment instead of political affairs, these concepts of zhengming and datong can become a proactive view of environmental ethics. From this view, zhengming emphasizes that ‘humans fulfill their responsibility of being humans’ (ren ren 人人). This means that humans should take responsibility toward the natural world, and one might say that ren ren implies an impetus for protecting the environment. Similarly, datong emphasizes caring for tian xia, by not limiting oneself to certain races, species, national boundaries, or geographical regions. Because of this, I believe that zhengming and datong can certainly resolve the above-mentioned problems: A lack of a sense of responsibility toward the environment, and a lack of a sense of care for what is outside of the confines of one’s immediate environment. 相似文献
333.
Jan Zirk‐Sadowski Charlotte Lamptey Amy Devine Mark Haggard Dénes Szűcs 《Developmental science》2014,17(3):366-375
We studied whether the origins of math anxiety can be related to a biologically supported framework of stress induction: (un)controllability perception, here indicated by self‐reported independent efforts in mathematics. Math anxiety was tested in 182 children (8‐ to 11‐year‐olds). Latent factor modeling was used to test hypotheses on plausible causal processes and mediations within competing models in quasi‐experimental contrasts. Uncontrollability perception in mathematics, or (in)dependence of efforts, best fit the data as an antecedent of math anxiety. In addition, the relationship of math anxiety with gender was fully mediated by adaptive perception of control (i.e. controllability). That is, young boys differ from girls in terms of their experience of control in mathematics learning. These differences influence math anxiety. Our findings are consistent with recent suggestions in clinical literature according to which uncontrollability makes women more susceptible to fear and anxiety disorders. A video abstract of this article can be viewed at http://www.youtube.com/watch?v=I4b_47Y-e_U 相似文献
334.
Jan te Nijenhuis Karen van Dam Sophie Aarts Henk van der Flier 《International Journal of Selection & Assessment》2014,22(1):88-100
Immigrant job applicants are often not fluent in the majority language. This raises the question whether selectors adjust their decision strategy when selecting immigrants. In two field studies (N = 1,949 and N = 156), we examined the decision strategies of professional selectors. Selectors used differential strategies, giving less weight to the test scores and impression scores of immigrants. Dutch language proficiency served as a moderator variable; test scores were considered less important in the employment recommendation especially for immigrants with low language skills. Yet professional selectors also used similar strategies for different groups; the same test scores led to the same recommendations, regardless of ethnicity. In sum, personnel selectors adjusted their decision strategy for immigrants, but this did not lead to immigrants receiving higher recommendations than majority applicants with similar test scores. 相似文献
335.
336.
337.
Kristofer Vernmark Jan Lenndin Mattias Carlsson Jörgen Öberg Thomas Eriksson 《Behaviour research and therapy》2010,48(5):368-376
Internet-delivered psychological treatment of major depression has been investigated in several trials, but the role of personalized treatment is less investigated. Studies suggest that guidance is important and that automated computerized programmes without therapist support are less effective. Individualized e-mail therapy for depression has not been studied in a controlled trial. Eighty-eight individuals with major depression were randomized to two different forms of Internet-delivered cognitive behaviour therapy (CBT), or to a waiting-list control group. One form of Internet treatment consisted of guided self-help, with weekly modules and homework assignments. Standard CBT components were presented and brief support was provided during the treatment. The other group received e-mail therapy, which was tailored and did not use the self-help texts i.e., all e-mails were written for the unique patient. Both treatments lasted for 8 weeks. In the guided self-help 93% completed (27/29) and in the e-mail therapy 96% (29/30) completed the posttreatment assessment. Results showed significant symptom reductions in both treatment groups with moderate to large effect sizes. At posttreatment 34.5% of the guided self-help group and 30% of the e-mail therapy group reached the criteria of high-end-state functioning (Beck Depression Inventory score below 9). At six-month follow-up the corresponding figures were 47.4% and 43.3%. Overall, the difference between guided self-help and e-mail therapy was small, but in favour of the latter. These findings indicate that both guided self-help and individualized e-mail therapy can be effective. 相似文献
338.
There is an anomaly in the hindsight bias literature with respect to hindsight effects obtained after self-relevant negative
event outcomes: Whereas some studies have reported reduced hindsight bias, others have shown increases. This article contrasts
two explanations for the anomaly. The first points to an influence of perceived control over the event outcome: In hindsight,
people decrease foreseeability (and hence, responsibility and blame) for controllable events, but they increase the perceived
inevitability of uncontrollable events for coping reasons. The second explanation, derived from a reconception of hindsight
bias in terms of separate components (Blank, Nestler, von Collani, & Fischer, 2008), traces the anomaly to differences in
the observed hindsight components: Hindsight decreases are to be expected for foreseeability, whereas increases are restricted
to the inevitability component. Our experiment (N=210) manipulated controllability and the hindsight component orthogonally and showed strong support for the component explanation,
but also some influence of perceived control. 相似文献
339.
340.
Abstract Fazio, Sanbonmatsu, Powell, and Kardes (1986) argued that affect may be activated automatically from memory on the mere observation of an affect-loaded stimulus. Using a variant of the standard sequential priming paradigm, it was demonstrated that the time needed to evaluate target words as positive or negative decreased if they were preceded by a similarly valenced prime word, but increased when preceded by a prime of opposite valence. Several aspects of their procedure, however, do not warrant their conclusion concerning the unconditionality of the effect. The present research investigated the generality of this affective priming effect. In Experiment 1, it was tested whether the effect can be generalised to more complex visual material. Stimulus pairs consisted of colour slides. Subjects had to evaluate the targets as quickly as possible. In Experiment 2, the standard word-word procedure was used, but target words had to be pronounced. In both experiments, significant affective priming effects were observed, supporting Bargh, Chaiken, Govender, and Pratto's (1992) assertion that the automatic activation effect is a pervasive and relative unconditional phenomenon. Implications for theories of affect and emotion are discussed. 相似文献