全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2054篇 |
免费 | 87篇 |
专业分类
2141篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 13篇 |
2022年 | 10篇 |
2021年 | 25篇 |
2020年 | 35篇 |
2019年 | 43篇 |
2018年 | 64篇 |
2017年 | 73篇 |
2016年 | 73篇 |
2015年 | 55篇 |
2014年 | 56篇 |
2013年 | 266篇 |
2012年 | 120篇 |
2011年 | 110篇 |
2010年 | 92篇 |
2009年 | 60篇 |
2008年 | 92篇 |
2007年 | 111篇 |
2006年 | 81篇 |
2005年 | 70篇 |
2004年 | 72篇 |
2003年 | 58篇 |
2002年 | 55篇 |
2001年 | 32篇 |
2000年 | 29篇 |
1999年 | 18篇 |
1998年 | 22篇 |
1997年 | 25篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 17篇 |
1993年 | 21篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 19篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 11篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 15篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 6篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1966年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有2141条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
131.
Karen van der Zee Jan Pieter van Oudenhoven Joseph G. Ponterotto Alexander W. Fietzer 《Journal of personality assessment》2013,95(1):118-124
This study reports on the development of the Multicultural Personality Questionnaire–Short Form among 511 participants. Using a split-sample scale validation design, Study 1 (N = 260) employed a principal component analysis and rigorous item selection criteria to extract a 40-item short form (MPQ–SF) from the original 91-item Multicultural Personality Questionnaire (MPQ; van der Zee & van Oudenhoven, 2000, 2001). In Study 2 (N = 251), the MPQ–SF was subjected to confirmatory factor analysis and resulted in a reasonably good fit to the data (comparative fit index = .94; root mean squared error of approximation = .066). Satisfactory coefficient alphas and high correlations with the original scales were found. Moreover, relationships with related scales were largely in the predicted direction. Specific directions for follow-up research are posited. 相似文献
132.
133.
134.
Jan Narveson 《The Journal of Ethics》2002,6(2):179-198
The basic bearer of responsibility is individuals, because that isall there are – nothing else can literally be the bearer of fullresponsibility. Claims about group responsibility therefore needanalysis. This would be impossible if all actions must be understoodas ones that could be performed whether or not anyone else exists.Individuals often act by virtue of membership in certain groups;often such membership bears a causal role in our behavior, andsometimes people act deliberately in order to promote the prospectsof members of a given group. Nevertheless, it is rational to awardproportionally to individual contributions to those actions andindividual shares in the production of the consequences of thoseactions. 相似文献
135.
Sarah J. Cotton Maureen F. Dollard Jan de Jonge 《International journal of stress management》2002,9(3):147-162
It is imperative that the university environment be supportive and capable of nurturing optimal learning and performance in students. Using job design and work stress theories, the study assessed relationships between psychosocial work characteristics, well-being and satisfaction, and performance in a random sample survey of Australian university students (N = 176). Methodological improvements were a time lag between survey and performance measures, an objective measure of performance, and LISREL structural equation modeling. Results showed high levels of psychological distress and low levels of satisfaction, both linked to high demands combined with low control. In accord with the happy-productive student hypothesis, satisfaction mediated the impact of the work environment on performance. Reengineering the design of the student work environment may therefore improve performance outcomes (student grades) through enhancing satisfaction. 相似文献
136.
Bonillo A Araujo Jiménez EA Jané Ballabriga MC Capdevila C Riera R 《Child neuropsychology》2012,18(4):347-355
The BRIEF-P is one of the most used instruments to measure Executive Function (EF). This report is aimed at showing the psychometric properties of the Catalan version of it. To do this, a random cluster sampling was carried out in Osona (Catalonia), recruiting 417 teachers and 408 parents of children aged 3 to 6 years. As with the original instrument, results show that reliability is excellent and that girls have a better EF than boys in the following areas: Working Memory, Plan/Organize, and Inhibit. Since in some areas Catalonia's preschool children show EF values slightly better than those of the reference sample, a new scale for using it in this population is provided. 相似文献
137.
Autism spectrum disorder (ASD) symptoms are present in unaffected relatives and individuals from the general population. Results are inconclusive, however, on whether unaffected relatives have higher levels of quantitative autism traits (QAT) or not. This might be due to differences in research populations, because behavioral data and molecular genetic research suggest that the genetic etiology of ASD is different in multiplex and simplex families. We compared 117 unaffected siblings and 276 parents of at least one child with ASD with 280 children and 595 adults from the general population on the presence of QAT using the Social Responsiveness Scale (SRS). Mean SRS scores for siblings, control children, parents and control adults were 25.4, 26.6, 33.7 and 32.9. Fathers of children with ASD showed significantly higher levels of QAT than controls, but siblings and mothers did not. We could not detect a statistically significant difference in SRS scores between relatives from simplex and multiplex families. These results do not support the theory of differential (genetic) etiology in multiplex and simplex families and suggest that a carried genetic risk is generally not expressed phenotypically in most relatives, except in fathers. 相似文献
138.
139.
Much previous research has demonstrated that visual search is typically disrupted by the presence of a unique “singleton”
distractor in the search display. Here we show that attentional capture by an irrelevant color singleton during shape search
critically depends on availability of working memory to the search task: When working memory is loaded in a concurrent yet
unrelated verbal short-term memory task, capture increases. These findings converge with previous demonstrations that increasing
working memory load results in greater distractor interference in Stroop-like tasks (de Fockert, Rees, Frith, & Lavie, 2001;
Lavie, Hirst, de Fockert, & Viding, 2004), which support the hypothesis that working memory provides goal-directed control
of visual selective attention allowing to minimize interference by goal-irrelevant distractors. 相似文献
140.
Jan De Houwer Tanja Geldof Els De Bruycker 《Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale》2005,59(4):228-239
The Implicit Association Test (IAT) is widely used as a measure of semantic similarity (i.e., associations in semantic memory). The results of previous research and of a new study show that IAT effects can, however, also be based on other types of similarity between stimuli. We therefore put forward the hypothesis that the IAT provides a general measure of similarity. Given that similarity is highly dynamic and context-dependent, our view that the IAT measures similarity is compatible with existing evidence showing that IAT effects are highly malleable. We provide further evidence for this in a new study in which the outcome of an IAT depended on whether the perceptual or functional characteristics of the stimuli were made salient. 相似文献