全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2427篇 |
免费 | 27篇 |
专业分类
2454篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 17篇 |
2023年 | 21篇 |
2022年 | 18篇 |
2021年 | 31篇 |
2020年 | 50篇 |
2019年 | 61篇 |
2018年 | 78篇 |
2017年 | 97篇 |
2016年 | 98篇 |
2015年 | 67篇 |
2014年 | 83篇 |
2013年 | 293篇 |
2012年 | 141篇 |
2011年 | 122篇 |
2010年 | 106篇 |
2009年 | 75篇 |
2008年 | 105篇 |
2007年 | 121篇 |
2006年 | 87篇 |
2005年 | 75篇 |
2004年 | 81篇 |
2003年 | 62篇 |
2002年 | 58篇 |
2001年 | 32篇 |
2000年 | 30篇 |
1999年 | 17篇 |
1998年 | 23篇 |
1997年 | 26篇 |
1996年 | 25篇 |
1995年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 19篇 |
1993年 | 21篇 |
1992年 | 27篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 19篇 |
1989年 | 19篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 19篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 10篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1980年 | 11篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 6篇 |
1976年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 6篇 |
1966年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有2454条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
Ivanouw J 《Journal of personality assessment》2007,89(Z1):S42-S51
A sample (n = 141) of Danish nonpatients 25-50 years of age, never hospitalized with a psychiatric diagnosis and currently employed, was demographically representative of two geographical areas of Copenhagen with different social strain. The sample, as well as 45 persons not currently employed, was tested with the Rorschach (Exner, 1995), MMPI-2 (Butcher, Dahlstrom, Graham, Tellegen, & Kaemmer, 1989), Word Association Test (Ivanouw, 1999b), WAIS Comprehension subtest (Hess, 1974), and SCL-90-R (Olsen, Mortenson, & Bech, 2006). Half of the persons contacted volunteered for the study. There was no difference in rate of volunteering between a standard no-feedback condition and a feedback condition; the latter, however, tended to attract more psychologically resourceful persons. The employed persons tended to appear healthier than the not employed. Response style of the subjects, quality of the Rorschach protocols, reliability of scoring, and the effect of the data being grouped on geographical area and examiner were examined. Form level, color, texture, cooperative movement, and EA were lower than in the Comprehensive System (CS; n = 450) sample, but higher than in nine international nonpatient Rorschach studies. Unique for the Danish sample was a low number of animal movement answers. The Rorschach data showed women to be healthier than men. Differences in Rorschach variables based on educational level were small. 相似文献
92.
93.
94.
Fear-related stimuli are often prioritized during visual selection but it remains unclear whether capture by salient objects is more likely to occur when individuals fear those objects. In this study, participants with high and low fear of spiders searched for a circle while on some trials a completely irrelevant fear-related (spider) or neutral distractor (butterfly/leaf) was presented simultaneously in the display. Our results show that when you fear spiders and you are not sure whether a spider is going to be present, then any salient distractor (i.e., a butterfly) grabs your attention, suggesting that mere expectation of a spider triggered compulsory monitoring of all irrelevant stimuli. However, neutral stimuli did not grab attention when high spider fearful people knew that a spider could not be present during a block of trials, treating the neutral stimuli just as the low spider fearful people do. Our results show that people that fear spiders inspect potential spider-containing locations in a compulsory fashion even though directing attention to this location is completely irrelevant for the task. Reduction of capture can only be accomplished when people that fear spiders do not expect a spider to be present. 相似文献
95.
Mantra is a free and open-source software package for object tracking. It is specifically designed to be used as a tool for
response collection in psychological experiments and requires only a computer and a camera (a webcam is sufficient). Mantra
is compatible with widely used software for creating psychological experiments. In Experiments 1 and 2, we validated the spatial and temporal precision of Mantra in realistic experimental settings. In Experiments 3 and 4, we validated the spatial precision and accuracy of Mantra more rigorously by tracking a computer controlled physical stimulus
and stimuli presented on a computer screen. 相似文献
96.
Jan Theeuwes 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1992,51(6):599-606
Three visual-search experiments tested whether the preattentive parallel stage can-selectively guide the attentive stage to a particular known-to-be-relevant target feature. Subjects searched multielement displays for a salient green circle that had a unique form when surrounded by green nontarget squares or had a unique color when surrounded by red nontarget circles. In the distractor conditions, a salient item in the other dimension was present as well. As an extension of earlier findings (Theeuwes, 1991), the results showed that complete top-down selectivity toward a particular feature was not possible, not even after extended and consistent practice. The results reveal that selectivity depends on the relative discriminability of the stimulus dimensions: the presence of an irrelevant item with a unique color interferes with parallel search for a unique form, and vice versa. 相似文献
97.
Raymond G. Miltenberger Jan Larson Mark Doerner Laurie Orvedal 《Behavioral Interventions》1992,7(1):23-34
Direct care and supervisory staff in five residential training programs for persons with mental retardation in North Dakota rated the acceptability of six staff management procedures. Overall, staff rated “Instructions”, “Instructions and Modeling”, “Feedback and Praise”, and “Instructions, Feedback, and Praise”, as significantly more acceptable than “Self-Management” and “Self-Management, Feedback and Praise”. There was a significant group by treatment interaction in which supervisors and direct care staff had significantly different acceptability rating for four of the six procedures. The results were further analyzed by group and implications and future research are discuessed. 相似文献
98.
A right-neglect patient with focal left-hemisphere damage to the posterior superior parietal lobe was assessed for numerical knowledge and tested on the bisection of numerical intervals and visual lines. The semantic and verbal knowledge of numbers was preserved, whereas the performance in numerical tasks that strongly emphasize the visuo-spatial layout of numbers (e.g. number bisection) was impaired. The behavioral pattern of error in the two bisection tasks mirrored the one previously described in left-neglect patients. In other words, our patient misplaced the subjective midpoint (numerical or visual) to the left as function of the interval size. These data, paired with the patient's lesion site are strictly consistent with the tripartite organization of number-related processes in the parietal lobes as proposed by Dehaene and colleagues. According to these authors, the posterior superior parietal lobe on both hemispheres underpins the attentional orientation on the putative mental number line, the horizontal segment of the intraparietal sulcus is bilaterally related to the semantic of the numerical domain, whereas the left angular gyrus subserves the verbal knowledge of numbers. In summary, our results suggest that the processes involved in the navigation along the mental number line, which are related to the parietal mechanisms for spatial attention, and the processes involved in the semantic and verbal knowledge of numbers, are dissociable. 相似文献
99.
ABSTRACT— To understand the mechanisms of visual attention, it is crucial to know the relationship between attention and saccades. Some theories propose a close relationship, whereas others view the attention and saccade systems as completely independent. One possible way to resolve this controversy is to distinguish between the maintenance and shifting of attention. The present study used a novel paradigm that allowed simultaneous measurement of attentional allocation and saccade preparation. Saccades toward the location where attention was maintained were either facilitated or suppressed depending on the probability of making a saccade to that location and the match between the attended location and the saccade location on the previous trial. Shifting attention to another location was always associated with saccade facilitation. The findings provide a new view, demonstrating that the maintenance of attention and shifting of attention differ in their relationship to the oculomotor system. 相似文献
100.
Heather L. Ramey Donato Tarulli Jan C. Frijters Lianne Fisher 《Contemporary Family Therapy》2009,31(4):262-279
Externalizing, or separating the person from his/her problem-saturated story, is a central approach in narrative therapy.
Michael White, one of the therapy’s founders, lately revised his map of the externalizing process in therapy according to
Vygotskian theory. In this study we sought to determine whether White’s proposed process was evident in therapy sessions.
Sequential analysis indicated that therapists scaffolded children’s responses according to White’s map, and therapists’ and
children’s utterances tended to advance across the levels of the map over the course of a session, indicating that White’s
model of narrative therapy matched the therapy’s empirical process. 相似文献