全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2276篇 |
免费 | 30篇 |
专业分类
2306篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 13篇 |
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 29篇 |
2020年 | 40篇 |
2019年 | 45篇 |
2018年 | 68篇 |
2017年 | 79篇 |
2016年 | 85篇 |
2015年 | 59篇 |
2014年 | 65篇 |
2013年 | 273篇 |
2012年 | 122篇 |
2011年 | 118篇 |
2010年 | 98篇 |
2009年 | 68篇 |
2008年 | 99篇 |
2007年 | 117篇 |
2006年 | 89篇 |
2005年 | 73篇 |
2004年 | 77篇 |
2003年 | 60篇 |
2002年 | 57篇 |
2001年 | 36篇 |
2000年 | 31篇 |
1999年 | 18篇 |
1998年 | 25篇 |
1997年 | 27篇 |
1996年 | 26篇 |
1995年 | 19篇 |
1994年 | 17篇 |
1993年 | 21篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 20篇 |
1989年 | 22篇 |
1988年 | 17篇 |
1987年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 9篇 |
1984年 | 10篇 |
1983年 | 11篇 |
1982年 | 21篇 |
1981年 | 14篇 |
1980年 | 12篇 |
1979年 | 8篇 |
1978年 | 17篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 10篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1966年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有2306条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
141.
Martin J. Meinhardt Raoul Bell Axel Buchner Jan P. Röer 《Psychonomic bulletin & review》2018,25(4):1399-1404
Animate entities are often better remembered than inanimate ones. The proximal mechanisms underlying this animacy effect on recall are unclear. In two experiments, we tested whether the animacy effect is due to emotional arousal. Experiment 1 revealed that translations of the animate words used in the pioneering study of Nairne et al. (Psychological science, 24, 2099–2105, 2013) were perceived as being more arousing than translations of the inanimate words, suggesting that animacy might have been confounded with arousal in previous studies. In Experiment 2, new word lists were created in which the animate and inanimate words were matched on arousal (amongst several other dimensions), and participants were required to reproduce the animate and inanimate words in a free recall task. There was a tendency towards better memory for arousing items, but robust animacy effects were obtained even though animate and inanimate words were matched on arousal. Thus, while arousal may contribute to the animacy effect when it is not carefully controlled for, it cannot explain the memory advantage of animate items. 相似文献
142.
Sixty-eight outpatients from a veterans' administration psychiatry clinic and community mental health center were assessed with 3 measures of depressive personality disorder (DPD)-the Diagnostic Interview for Depressive Personality Disorder (Gunderson, Phillips, Triebwasser, &; Hirschfeld, 1994), the Depressive Personality Disorder Inventory (Huprich, Margrett, Barthelemy, &; Fine, 1996), and the Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV Axis II Disorders (First, Gibbon, Spitzer, Williams, &; Benjamin, 1997a)-to evaluate their convergent and discriminant validity. Evidence supporting the measures' validity was mixed. The rate of convergence of depressive personality diagnoses across 3 measures was less than optimal, but the degree of intercorrelation among the measures was strong. Although depressive personality scores had moderate levels of intercorrelations with other personality disorders, the degree of intercorrelation decreased substantially after controlling for depressive symptoms. I conclude that further work is needed to strengthen the validity of measures of DPD. 相似文献
143.
144.
Shriram Ramanathan Paul C. McIntyre Jan Luning Piero Pianetta David A. Muller 《Philosophical Magazine Letters》2013,93(9):519-528
We present detailed structural and chemical studies on ultrathin zirconia films grown by ultraviolet oxidation and natural (no ultraviolet light) oxidation of Zr precursor metal layers on SiO2-passivated Si(100) wafers. Quantitative electron-energy-loss spectroscopy (EELS) was used to obtain the chemical composition and electronic structure on an atomic scale. The EELS OK near-edge fine structure was used as a fingerprint to study the stoichiometry and composition variation in the dielectric stacks. X-ray absorption spectroscopy studies were performed in order to aid interpretation of the EELS results. The electronic structure data are correlated with the electrical performance of the films. 相似文献
145.
146.
Over the past two decades, research has accumulated compelling evidence that sleep supports the formation of long-term memory.
The standard two-stage memory model that has been originally elaborated for declarative memory assumes that new memories are
transiently encoded into a temporary store (represented by the hippocampus in the declarative memory system) before they are
gradually transferred into a long-term store (mainly represented by the neocortex), or are forgotten. Based on this model,
we propose that sleep, as an offline mode of brain processing, serves the ‘active system consolidation’ of memory, i.e. the
process in which newly encoded memory representations become redistributed to other neuron networks serving as long-term store.
System consolidation takes place during slow-wave sleep (SWS) rather than rapid eye movement (REM) sleep. The concept of active
system consolidation during sleep implicates that (a) memories are reactivated during sleep to be consolidated, (b) the consolidation
process during sleep is selective inasmuch as it does not enhance every memory, and (c) memories, when transferred to the
long-term store undergo qualitative changes. Experimental evidence for these three central implications is provided: It has
been shown that reactivation of memories during SWS plays a causal role for consolidation, that sleep and specifically SWS
consolidates preferentially memories with relevance for future plans, and that sleep produces qualitative changes in memory
representations such that the extraction of explicit and conscious knowledge from implicitly learned materials is facilitated. 相似文献
147.
Sensorimotor prediction and memory in object manipulation. 总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5
J R Flanagan S King D M Wolpert R S Johansson 《Revue canadienne de psychologie expérimentale》2001,55(2):87-95
When people lift objects of different size but equal weight, they initially employ too much force for the large object and too little force for the small object. However, over repeated lifts of the two objects, they learn to suppress the size-weight association used to estimate force requirements and appropriately scale their lifting forces to the true and equal weights of the objects. Thus, sensorimotor memory from previous lifts comes to dominate visual size information in terms of force prediction. Here we ask whether this sensorimotor memory is transient, preserved only long enough to perform the task, or more stable. After completing an initial lift series in which they lifted equally weighted large and small objects in alternation, participants then repeated the lift series after delays of 15 minutes or 24 hours. In both cases, participants retained information about the weights of the objects and used this information to predict the appropriate fingertip forces. This preserved sensorimotor memory suggests that participants acquired internal models of the size-weight stimuli that could be used for later prediction. 相似文献
148.
Frank Baeyens Paul Eelen Geert Crombez Jan De Houwer 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2001,20(2):183-203
In previous research (Baeyens, Vansteenwegen et al., 1996) we demonstrated that when observers consume a series of CS+ and CS−flavored drinks while simultaneously watching
a videotaped model who synchronically drinks identical drinks and facially expresses his evaluation (dislike to CS+, neutral
to CS−) of the liquids, the observers acquire a dislike for CS+ flavored relative to CS−flavored drinks. The aim of the present
experiments was to test some predictions derived from a “direct conditioning” theory of such observational flavor learning.
Using the same observational flavor conditioning procedure, we investigated (Exp. 1) the effect of manipulating the observers’
belief concerning the relationship between the drinks that they and the model were consuming (same/different/no information).
Observational flavor conditioning was obtained when observers were led to believe that they were drinking the same drinks
as the model did, and when they were not informed about this relationship, but not when told to be drinking different drinks.
At the same time, however, the observers were not able to correctly identify the source of the model’s expression of dislike:
They showed no CS-US contingency-awareness. Whereas the former finding suggests the causal involvement of conscious beliefs
and cognitive inference processes in observational learning, the latter is more in line with the idea that the model’s facial
expressions may act like a US’ which is automatically associated with the paired flavor CS+, without any involvement of conscious
beliefs or cognitive inferences. These two crucial findings were replicated in Exp. 2. Also, we obtained evidence in this
study that the belief manipulation affected learning through its influence on the observers’ attention for the model’s facial
evaluative expressions. These results can be integrated either by a cognitive theory allowing the beliefs on which the inferences
are based to be of an implicit nature, or by a “direct conditioning” theory that conceives of the US’ as an interpreted event,
rather than as a mechanistically and invariantly acting physical entity. 相似文献
149.
Jan Zygmunt 《逻辑史和逻辑哲学》2013,34(1-2):41-53
This paper describes the work of the Polish logician Jan Kalicki (1922–1953). After a biographical introduction, his work on logical matrices and equational logic is appraised. A bibliography of his papers and reviews is also included. 相似文献
150.
Previous research has shown that emotional stimuli interfere with ongoing activities. One explanation is that these stimuli
draw attention away from the primary task and thereby hamper the correct execution of the task. Another explanation is that
emotional stimuli cause a temporary freezing of all ongoing activity. We used a go/ no-go task to differentiate between these
accounts. According to the attention account, emotional distractors should impair performance on both go and no-go trials.
According to the freezing account, the presentation of emotional stimuli should be detrimental to performance on go trials,
but beneficial for performance on no-go trials. Our findings confirm the former prediction: Pictures high in emotional arousal
impaired performance on no-go trials. 相似文献