首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2186篇
  免费   90篇
  2023年   12篇
  2022年   16篇
  2021年   31篇
  2020年   37篇
  2019年   47篇
  2018年   72篇
  2017年   79篇
  2016年   76篇
  2015年   58篇
  2014年   59篇
  2013年   294篇
  2012年   122篇
  2011年   122篇
  2010年   94篇
  2009年   66篇
  2008年   101篇
  2007年   115篇
  2006年   85篇
  2005年   72篇
  2004年   73篇
  2003年   61篇
  2002年   56篇
  2001年   36篇
  2000年   31篇
  1999年   18篇
  1998年   23篇
  1997年   25篇
  1996年   25篇
  1995年   20篇
  1994年   17篇
  1993年   22篇
  1992年   25篇
  1991年   12篇
  1990年   19篇
  1989年   19篇
  1988年   17篇
  1987年   18篇
  1985年   10篇
  1984年   11篇
  1983年   11篇
  1982年   21篇
  1981年   13篇
  1980年   10篇
  1979年   7篇
  1978年   15篇
  1976年   8篇
  1974年   7篇
  1971年   8篇
  1970年   7篇
  1966年   8篇
排序方式: 共有2276条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
961.
Computers and girls: Rethinking the issues   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Jan Hawkins 《Sex roles》1985,13(3-4):165-180
A perspective is developed about emerging sex differences in the use of computer technology in education. Computers tend to be conceptually assimilated to the category of science/mathematics/technology, and thus acquire some of the traditional qualities of differentiated interest among boys and girls. Findings from several research projects are reported which point to patterns of difference in educational settings. It is argued that sex differences emerge in relation to the functions computers serve and the organization of learning settings of which they are a part.The preparation of this article and the research reported was supported by the Spencer Foundation, the Department of Education, and the National Institute of Education. The author would like to thank Kathy Clement, Carla Freeman, Peggy Heide, Moni Homolsky, Midian Kurland, Ron Mawby, Roy Pea, and Karen Sheingold for their comments and suggestions about the perspective presented here.  相似文献   
962.
963.
964.
965.
Jan Srzednicki 《Axiomathes》1997,8(1-3):293-306
  相似文献   
966.
967.
968.
969.
We report on two studies in which we measured army cadets' tendencies to engage in two types of self‐enhancement: communal self‐enhancement (a moralistic bias in self‐presentation) and agentic self‐enhancement (an egotistic bias in self‐presentation). These self‐presentation styles were then related to their selection and performance as military leaders. In Study 1, scores on self‐enhancement questionnaires were used to predict selection decisions for 206 applicants to an army officer training program. We found that applicants who were higher in either communal or agentic self‐enhancement were more likely to be accepted for leadership training. In Study 2, we evaluated peer and superior ratings of 94 military cadets' leadership, reflecting leadership emergence and leadership effectiveness, respectively. We found that communal self‐enhancement negatively predicted leadership emergence, with those ratings becoming more negative over a 3‐year time‐span, whereas agentic self‐enhancement positively predicted leadership effectiveness. Our results imply that, at least in the present military context, people making selection decisions should be particularly aware of the relations between (a) applicant self‐enhancement tendencies and those decisions, and (b) high communal self‐enhancement in officer trainees and negative evaluations by their cadet peers.  相似文献   
970.
It is well known that a gender difference in physical aggression emerges by the preschool years. We tested the hypothesis that the gender difference is partly due to changing tactics in peer interaction. Observations of girls’ and boys’ social initiatives and reactions to opportunities for conflict were made, using the Peer Interaction Coding System (PICS) in four independent samples of children between 9 and 36 months of age, which were aggregated to form a summary data set (N= 323), divided into two age bands (below or above 24 months of age). Linear mixed‐model analyses revealed significant age by gender interactions in the use of bodily force in response to peers’ initiatives and in the tendency to use bodily force at later stages of conflicts with peers. The gender difference in use of force was not explained by differences in the use of verbal tactics. These cross‐sectional findings suggest that girls are initially more likely than boys to use reactive aggression, but then desist, whereas boys increase their use of force to defend their territory and possessions. The difference between older and younger girls likely reflects girls’ abilities to regulate their behaviour in response to social challenges and the fact that girls are explicitly socialized to yield to peers’ demands.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号