全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1109篇 |
免费 | 49篇 |
专业分类
1158篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 7篇 |
2022年 | 10篇 |
2021年 | 11篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 27篇 |
2018年 | 45篇 |
2017年 | 30篇 |
2016年 | 39篇 |
2015年 | 26篇 |
2014年 | 22篇 |
2013年 | 131篇 |
2012年 | 54篇 |
2011年 | 64篇 |
2010年 | 31篇 |
2009年 | 27篇 |
2008年 | 49篇 |
2007年 | 48篇 |
2006年 | 50篇 |
2005年 | 44篇 |
2004年 | 35篇 |
2003年 | 22篇 |
2002年 | 37篇 |
2001年 | 17篇 |
2000年 | 28篇 |
1999年 | 14篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 17篇 |
1996年 | 16篇 |
1995年 | 7篇 |
1994年 | 11篇 |
1993年 | 13篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 11篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 6篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1985年 | 10篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 9篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 7篇 |
1975年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 6篇 |
1973年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1970年 | 6篇 |
1966年 | 8篇 |
排序方式: 共有1158条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Miller and Paredes (1990) found that both children's and adults' performance of simple addition and multiplication tested in mixed-operation blocks was slower and more susceptible to operation errors (e.g., 6 + 2=12 or 6×2=8) compared to pure-operation blocks; but mixed-operation costs were greater for addition than for multiplication. We pursued the source of these effects in adults (n=96) by examining effects of operation switching and cueing. We replicated the asymmetrical costs of mixing addition and multiplication and demonstrated that these owed, in part, to asymmetrical task switching costs. Operation foreknowledge (a visual cue appearing 2 s before the problem) moderated both mixing and switch costs. This suggests that an asymmetry between addition and multiplication in the utility of preparation contributed to asymmetric mixing costs. Miller and Paredes proposed that the asymmetric costs of mixing operations observed in adults echoed an asymmetry developed in childhood when multiplication is initially learned. This might be correct, but our results raise the possibility that the asymmetry reflects operation-specific costs and interference effects that arise during the experiment. 相似文献
992.
Using an Imagery Intervention to Train Visual Exploratory Activity in Elite Academy Football Players
Chris Pocock Matt Dicks Richard C. Thelwell Michael Chapman Jamie B. Barker 《Journal of Applied Sport Psychology》2013,25(2):218-234
Football players adapt their movements to opportunities within the surrounding environment by engaging in visual exploratory activity (VEA) to pick up information. This study adds to the extant literature by using a 6-week PETTLEP imagery intervention to train VEA and improve performance with the ball. A single-case, multiple baseline across participants' design was conducted with 5 elite academy football players. Results indicated that a PETTLEP imagery intervention improved VEA, particularly in center midfielders. In addition, indications of improvements in performance with the ball were present within some participants. Future researchers could examine the processes underpinning VEA to enhance applied interventions for this skill. 相似文献
993.
Robert Ward Burnham 《The Journal of psychology》2013,147(1):79-89
Simulated and behavioral personal space measures and the Sensation Seeking Scale were administered to 20 males and 20 females. In the simulated measures Ss positioned male and female top profiles relative to a top profile representing themselves at each of nine equally spaced angles. For the behavioral measure Ss were approached by a male or female at each of the nine angles. The female “swinger” had greater simulated personal space. Males who liked new and interesting experiences had greater side and diagonal simulated personal space, and females high on the same scale had a closer behavioral personal space toward males except at the sides. Males and females responded similarly to males and females approaching from various directions. However, there was no relation between front and side simulated personal space for males. Size of personal space relative to females tended to be highly correlated with size of personal space toward males. However, for male Ss there was little relation between front personal space toward males and females. For male Ss simulated personal space and behavioral personal space were highly correlated. For female Ss the two measures were almost completely unrelated. 相似文献
994.
We report seven experiments in which subjects were trained to respond with numbers to the loudness of 1000-Hz pure tones according to power functions with exponents of 0.60, 0.30, and 0.90. Subjects were then presented with stimuli from other continua (65-Hz pure tones or 565-nm lights varying in amplitude) and were asked to judge the subjective magnitude of these stimuli on the same numerical scale. Stimuli from the training continuum were presented, with feedback, on every other trial in order to maintain the trained scale. Except for the 0.90 scale, subjects readily learned the predetermined scales and were able to use them to judge the non-training stimuli with group results consistent with those usually reported. Also, in contrast to the usual magnitude estimation results, these results produced extremely low levels of intersubject variability. We argue that such learned scales can be used as "rulers" for measuring perceived magnitudes, according to a common unit. 相似文献
995.
Jamie Zabukovec Steve Lazrove Francine Shapiro 《Journal of Psychotherapy Integration》2000,10(2):189-206
Eye Movement Desensitization and Reprocessing (EMDR) (Shapiro 1989a, 1989b, 1995) is an innovative, comprehensive approach to psychotherapy. While EMDR's use of eye movements has attracted a great deal of attention, the efficacy of the EMDR method can be explained parsimoniously in terms of many different types of therapy. Lang's (1985) information processing networks provide a way to understand the Accelerated Information Processing model proposed by Shapiro to explain EMDR. A representative EMDR session is presented to illustrate the integrative components of EMDR's procedural elements and the range of clinical effects. Therapeutic changes seen as a result of self-healing using EMDR are discussed from the perspectives of other psychotherapeutic approaches in order to understand the contribution of EMDR to the psychotherapy integration movement. 相似文献
996.
Simple addition (e.g., 3 + 2, 7 + 9) may be performed by direct memory retrieval or by such procedures as counting or transformation. The distribution of associations (DOA) model of strategy choice (Siegler, 1988) predicts that procedure use should increase as retrieval interference increases. To test this, 100 undergraduates performed simple addition problems, either after blocks of simple multiplication (high-interference context) or after blocks of simple division problems (low-interference context). Addition took longer and was more error prone after multiplication; in particular, there were more multiplication confusion errors on the relatively easy, small-number addition problems (e.g., 3 + 2 = 6, 4 + 3 = 12), but not on the more difficult, large-number additions. Consistent with the DOA, participants reported greater use of procedures for addition after multiplication, but more so for small addition problems. The findings demonstrate that adults’ use of procedural strategies for simple addition is substantially influenced by retrieval interference. 相似文献
997.
Ward E. Jones 《国际科学哲学研究》2000,14(3):299-304
After a thorough examination of the claim that “the underdetermination of theory by evidence forces us to seek sociological explanations of scientists' cognitive choices”, Samir Okasha concludes that the only significant problem with this argument is that the thesis of underdetermination is not adequately supported. Against Okasha, I argue (1) that there is a very good reason to question the inference from the underdetermination of a theory to a sociological account of that theory's acceptance, and (2) that Okasha's own objection to the argument is too weak. 相似文献
998.
Chad L. NeSmith S. Allen Wilcoxon Jamie F. Satcher 《Journal of Addictions & Offender Counseling》2000,20(2):75-83
This study examined the premise that women's addiction groups should be facilitated by female leaders. Results from pretest‐posttest comparisons indicated that attitudes toward men of participants in groups facilitated by men were more positive than were those of participants in groups facilitated by women. 相似文献
999.
Michael J. Kofler Jamie A. Spiegel Kristin E. Austin Lauren N. Irwin Elia F. Soto Dustin E. Sarver 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2018,46(6):1171-1185
Working memory deficits are present in a substantial proportion of children with ADHD, and converging evidence links these deficits with ADHD-related behavioral and functional impairments. At the same time, working memory is not a unitary construct, and evidence is lacking regarding the role of several components of this system in ADHD. Preclinical behavioral studies are needed to fractionate the multicomponent working memory system, determine which specific subcomponent(s) are impaired in ADHD, and more importantly link these subcomponent(s) with specific ADHD-related behavioral symptoms/functional impairments. The current study reflects one piece of that puzzle, and focuses on the episodic buffer component of working memory. Across multiple testing days, a well-characterized sample of 86 children ages 8–13 (M=10.52, SD=1.54; 34 girls; 64% Caucasian/Non-Hispanic) with ADHD (n=49) and without ADHD (n=37) completed three counterbalanced working memory tests that were identical in all aspects except the key subcomponent process (phonological, visuospatial, episodic buffer). Gross motor movement during these and control tasks were measured using 4 high-precision actigraphs. There was no evidence of group differences in gender, age, SES, or IQ. Bayesian mixed-model ANOVAs indicated that the ADHD group performed significantly worse on all three working memory tests (d=1.17–1.44) and was significantly more hyperactive than controls (d=0.66–1.05) during the visuospatial and episodic buffer tests. In contrast, the ADHD and Non-ADHD groups were equivalent with regard to effects of episodic buffer demands on performance and hyperactive behavior. The most parsimonious conclusion is that the episodic buffer is likely intact in ADHD, and unrelated to ADHD hyperactivity symptoms. 相似文献
1000.
Prior research has suggested that episodes of mind wandering not only negatively impact text comprehension but also are associated with fluctuations in reading behavior. However, these studies typically do not account for differences in the fundamental nature of the text itself, namely, whether it is narrative or expository in structure. As much research has supported the idea that these text genres are processed differently, it is of interest to determine whether similar changes in reading patterns are observed when mind wandering in an expository text. The present study examined whether fluctuations in sentence‐by‐sentence reading times were associated with periods of mindless reading during processing of an expository text. Results indicated that although mindless reading did negatively impact learning, probed reading time did not vary as a function of mind wandering. These results suggest that research aimed at studying mind wandering while reading may need to account for text genre. 相似文献