全文获取类型
收费全文 | 8405篇 |
免费 | 320篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
8726篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 103篇 |
2019年 | 141篇 |
2018年 | 157篇 |
2017年 | 193篇 |
2016年 | 216篇 |
2015年 | 158篇 |
2014年 | 203篇 |
2013年 | 914篇 |
2012年 | 326篇 |
2011年 | 318篇 |
2010年 | 234篇 |
2009年 | 214篇 |
2008年 | 311篇 |
2007年 | 294篇 |
2006年 | 280篇 |
2005年 | 253篇 |
2004年 | 264篇 |
2003年 | 243篇 |
2002年 | 293篇 |
2001年 | 105篇 |
2000年 | 102篇 |
1999年 | 128篇 |
1998年 | 138篇 |
1997年 | 125篇 |
1996年 | 111篇 |
1995年 | 132篇 |
1994年 | 109篇 |
1993年 | 102篇 |
1992年 | 107篇 |
1991年 | 100篇 |
1990年 | 90篇 |
1989年 | 82篇 |
1988年 | 66篇 |
1987年 | 66篇 |
1986年 | 69篇 |
1985年 | 85篇 |
1984年 | 106篇 |
1983年 | 119篇 |
1982年 | 115篇 |
1981年 | 144篇 |
1980年 | 104篇 |
1979年 | 121篇 |
1978年 | 125篇 |
1977年 | 118篇 |
1976年 | 102篇 |
1975年 | 103篇 |
1974年 | 105篇 |
1973年 | 66篇 |
1972年 | 63篇 |
1969年 | 56篇 |
排序方式: 共有8726条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
11.
This article presents a simulation-based tutorial system for exploring parallel distributed processing (PDP) models of information processing. The system consists of software and an accompanying handbook. The intent of the package is to make the ideas underlying PDP accessible and to disseminate some of the main simulation programs that we have developed. This article presents excerpts from the handbook that describe the approach taken, the organization of the handbook, and the software that comes with it. An example is given that illustrates the approach we have taken to teaching PDP, which involves presentation of relevant mathematical background, together with tutorial exercises that make use of the simulation programs. 相似文献
12.
13.
Three experiments were conducted to examine the operation of the representativeness and anchoring and adjustment heuristics in lottery play. Subjects in Experiments 1 and 2 indicated their chances of winning a lottery with an objective probability of 1 in 10. Consistent with the anchoring and adjustment heuristic, subjects (in both experiments) perceived their chances of winning to be greater when the lottery was based on a single event than when it was based on a disjunctive event. Subjects in these two experiments also selected numbers to play in a pick-3 (Experiment 1) or pick-4 (Experiment 2) lottery. Consistent with the representativeness heuristic, subjects in Experiment 2 demonstrated a preference for numbers without repeating digits. This also occurred in Experiment 3 wherein the numbers actually played in the Indiana daily Pick-3 lottery were examined. 相似文献
14.
A series of experiments investigated whether people could integrate nonspatial information about an object with their knowledge of the object's location in space. In Experiments 1 and 3, subjects learned the locations of cities on a fictitious road map; in Experiments 2, 4, and 5, subjects were already familiar with the locations of buildings on a campus. The subjects then learned facts about the cities on the maps or the buildings on the campus. The question of interest was whether or not these nonspatial facts would be integrated in memory with the spatial knowledge. After learning the facts, subjects were given a location-judgment test in which they had to decide whether an object was in one region of the space or another. Knowledge integration was assessed by comparing levels of performance in two conditions: (a) when a city or a building name was primed by a fact about a neighboring city or building, and (b) when a city or a building name was primed by a fact about a distant city or building. Results showed that responses in Condition a were faster or more accurate, or both faster and more accurate, than responses in Condition b. These results indicate that the spatial and nonspatial information were encoded in a common memory representation. 相似文献
15.
James R. Stagray David Downs Ronald K. Sommers 《Journal of psycholinguistic research》1992,21(6):511-524
We measured subjective-tone identification thresholds evoked by acoustically filtered complex tones and compared them to Mandarin-Chinese tone-phoneme identification thresholds previously measured under the same filtering conditions. Tone phonemes were identified at intensities below subjective-tone identification thresholds, suggesting subjective tone is not necessary for tone-phoneme identification. Lower subjective-tone thresholds evoked by resolved harmonics rather than unresolved harmonics were consistent with pattern recognition theories of pitch perception.The authors thank Donald Gans and Richard Klich for all their help. 相似文献
16.
This article describes a study conducted to identify the range of potential influences that a computer-assisted careers guidance system (PROSPECT (HE)) had on the functioning of organisations (careers services in higher education). Data from six institutions indicated that use of PROSPECT (HE) was associated with important and varied change within the organisations. It appears that when a careers service seeks opportunities for change, CACG systems can be an important catalyst for change in two respects: by providing a focal point for achieving previously established goals; or, as a consequence of system use, by making staff aware of the potential for new organisational goals. 相似文献
17.
James E. Gruber 《Sex roles》1992,26(11-12):447-464
Most of the research conducted on sexual harassment over the last decade and a half has used categories that are neither mutually exclusive nor exhaustive. This has created problems for researchers: it is difficult to compare results from one study to another, harassment types that have scholarly and legal-policy relevance are omitted, and the ability of researchers to inform legal and policy decisions is diminished as a result of these problems. A comprehensive categorization of harassment types that addresses these methodological problems is presented. Specifically, 11 specific types of harassment—4 types of Verbal Requests, 3 Verbal Remarks, and 4 Nonverbal Displays—are presented with examples from research and legal literatures. Recommendations for reconceptualizing research definitions of harassment as well as for diversifying the methodological approaches to the topic are made. 相似文献
18.
19.
James B. Freeman 《Argumentation》1992,6(2):219-275
20.
Quantification of a chaotic system can be made by calculating the correlation dimension (D2) of the data that the system generates
(Packard et al., 1980). The D2 algorithm, however, requires stationarity of the generator, a feature that biological data
rarely reflect (Mayer-Kress et al., 1988). So we developed the “point correlation dimension” (PD2), an algorithm that accurately
tracks D2 in linked data of different dimensions (Carpeggiani et al., 1991). We now present a mathematical argument that,
for stationary data, individual PD2s converge to D2 and we demonstrate that the algorithm rejects contributions made by bursts
of noise. Data were obtained from the surface of the olfactory bulb of the conscious rabbit (64 electrodes, 640 Hz each, 1.3
sec epochs) before and after presentation of a novel or habituated odor. D2 could be calculated in only 1 of 10 novel-odor
trials, whereas PD2 could be calculated in all. Both algorithms indicated that a novel odor evokes a spatially uniform dimensional
increase. The PD2 uniquely exhibited the dimensional decreases that occur during inspiration and the gradients of mean dimension
present during the nonstimulated control state. These control gradients remained unchanged without odor experience, but showed
spatially specific PD2 increases following odor habituation. It is interpreted that, 1) the PD2 issensitive, accurate, and appropriate for dimensional assessment of biological data, 2) that during analysis of unfamiliar information a singleglobal process is transiently evoked in the neuropil, and 3) after experience multiplespatially specific processes tonically map the sites of learning.
Grant Support: National Institutes of Health, HL 31164 and NS27745 相似文献