首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   23359篇
  免费   989篇
  国内免费   8篇
  2020年   268篇
  2019年   308篇
  2018年   417篇
  2017年   425篇
  2016年   477篇
  2015年   359篇
  2014年   455篇
  2013年   1970篇
  2012年   779篇
  2011年   735篇
  2010年   516篇
  2009年   482篇
  2008年   691篇
  2007年   695篇
  2006年   626篇
  2005年   598篇
  2004年   551篇
  2003年   507篇
  2002年   579篇
  2001年   772篇
  2000年   761篇
  1999年   578篇
  1998年   298篇
  1997年   278篇
  1996年   265篇
  1995年   258篇
  1994年   234篇
  1992年   460篇
  1991年   433篇
  1990年   439篇
  1989年   377篇
  1988年   352篇
  1987年   343篇
  1986年   321篇
  1985年   342篇
  1984年   338篇
  1983年   312篇
  1982年   262篇
  1981年   268篇
  1979年   340篇
  1978年   288篇
  1977年   259篇
  1976年   227篇
  1975年   290篇
  1974年   330篇
  1973年   295篇
  1972年   244篇
  1969年   230篇
  1968年   255篇
  1967年   224篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
941.
Although geometric information is altered when a picture’s viewing point is changed, such changes often do not affect perception. Two experiments assessed pictorial perception relative to the distortions introduced by viewing point dislocation. Results provide a psychophysical demonstration of pictorial compensation and suggest that it is based on the discrepancy between the actual and an assumed-correct viewing position. An explanation of pictorial compensation is offered that could be applied to direct picture perception and to picture-in-a-picture perception.  相似文献   
942.
The process of olfactory sensation is probably a Gestalt consisting of motor, physical, and chemical components formed during the transport of the odor-bearing airstream past the olfactory epithelium. In studying a sensory process, it is often necessary to examine the components independently. Apart from its contribution to the olfactory Gestalt, in dynamic olfactometry airflow may distract from, compete with, or mask chemical sensation. Four nonsmoking male subjects were employed to obtain flow thresholds of nonodorous air at eight temperature/ humidity combinations. The stimulus-generating apparatus was housed in a temperature-regulated oven. All materials in contact with the airstream were thoroughly cleaned and were constructed of nonreactive materials to avoid olfactory cues. An evaluation of the quality of the output demonstrated purity. A Béésy technique was used to deliver stimuli via a nasal catheter to the subject. Thresholds were found to be nearly identical for the lowest humidity levels regardless of the temperature. In general, the closer the temperature/humidity was to physiological normal, the higher the threshold. Since the thresholds were so strongly functions of temperature and humidity, it is likely that transduction was by trigeminal thermoreceptive afferents. The utilization of subthreshold flows in olfactory research may reduce the trigeminal component in olfaction, thus permitting better assessment of the contribution of the olfactory nerve to olfactory sensation.  相似文献   
943.
Five obliquely rotated factor analyses were performed on items from Eysenck and Eysenck’s (1975) EPQ, Zuckerman’s (1975) Sensation-Seeking Scale, and Strelau’s (1972) temperament inventory (STI), administered to 277 subjects (Ss). The analyses were used to examine the relationships between the personality dimensions E, N, L, P and Sensation-Seeking, which pertain mainly to social situations, attitudes and feelings, and questionnaire-derived measures of nervous system properties, which, while validated on experimental indices of nervous system properties derived from Nebylitsyn’s (1972) model, are expressed as characteristic features of social behavior, work style and activity. Four factors were identified—extraversion/strength of excitation/mobility; self-control of affect/stability/caution; strength of inhibition/verbal control/motor expressiveness/ nonmanipulativeness; and sensation-seeking/nervousness. The results offer support for Eysenck’s claim of some identity between excitatory strength and E, for the reported relationship between E and mobility, and for Eysenck’s conditioning postulate. They also suggest that similar temperamental variables underlie individual differences in both social and occupational/motor activity. Since Strelau’s STI items are validated on experimental indices of nervous system properties, it follows that typological characteristics may be inferred from questionnaire data derived from both behavioral categories.  相似文献   
944.
A special-purpose interval-duration timer is described. The device is designed to interface with commercially available psychophysical packages, to be used in time interval calibration. The extremely low cost of materials justifies the construction of this essentially single-purpose device.  相似文献   
945.
Subjects classified sets of multidimensional stimuli into two groups in any way they wished. The sets were composed of 6 or 12 stimuli: 2 or 4 instances of 3 different stimuli (e.g., 2 blue circles, 2 green circles, 2 red circles). There were striking individual differences in the preferred classification. Some subjects maximized the similarity between subgroups by matching the composition of the subgroups--one instance of each stimulus was placed in each group. The other subjects maximized the similarity among stimuli within each subgroup by placing similar stimuli in each group (the blues and greens in one group, the reds in the other). The nature of the stimuli as well as the relationships among the three stimuli had little effect on classification. In this case, cognitive styles specific to individuals but general across diverse dimensions and stimulus sets determined classification.  相似文献   
946.
Recent experiments have indicated that contrast effects can be obtained with vowels by anchoring a test series with one of the endpoint vowels. These contextual effects cannot be attributed to feature detector fatigue or to the induction of an overt response bias. In the present studies, anchored ABX discrimination functions and signal detection analyses of identification data (before and after anchoring) for an [i]-[I] vowel series were used to demonstrate that [i] and [I] anchoring produce contrast effects by affecting different perceptual mechanisms. The effects of [i] anchoring were to increase within-[i] category sensitivity, while [I] anchoring shifted criterion placements. When vowels were placed in CVC syllables to reduce available auditory memory, there was a significant decrease in the size of the [I]-anchor contrast effects. The magnitude of the [i]-anchor effect was unaffected by the reduction in vowel information available in auditory memory. These results suggest that [i] and [I] anchors affect mechanisms at different levels of processing. The [i] anchoring results may reflect normalization processes in speech perception that operate at an early level of perceptual processing, while the [I] anchoring results represent changes in response criterion mediated by auditory memory for vowel information.  相似文献   
947.
The variable that affect motor programming time may be studied by changing the nature of the response and measuring the subsequent changes in reaction time (RT). One notion of motor programming suggests that aiming responses with reduced target size and/or increased target amplitude require more "complex" motor programs that require longer RTs. In a series of five experiments which movement time (MT) was experimentally varied target size neither influences RT when the movement amplitude was 2 or 30 cm nor when the target sizes differed by as much as a factor of 16:1. Increasing the movement amplitude from 15 to 30 cm also had no influence on RT. Movement time, however, did affect RT, with 200-msec movements having longer RTs than 120-msec movements. Target size and movement amplitude did not appear to be factors that influence programming time or program complexity.  相似文献   
948.
The difference between the Henry "memory-drum" theory and our version is that ours includes, an additional assumption that, after programming has occurred, the resultant representation can be stored in short-term memory. Otherwise, the essential ideas are the same in the two theories. Implications of the presently available data for the distinction between the theories are discussed. Regardless of how one evaluates our added assumption, it is clear that the essential insight of the Henry theory has fared very well in the 20 years since the theory first appeared in print.  相似文献   
949.
Two experiments investigated the social desirability of Bem Sex-Role Inventory (BSRI) items. Mean social desirability of the masculinity subscale was significantly higher than that of the femininity subscale in both experiments. A revision of the BSRI femininity subscale was suggested which replaced three items of questionable social value with three more socially desirable items and virtually eliminated the social desirability difference between masculinity and femininity subscales.  相似文献   
950.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号