全文获取类型
收费全文 | 39341篇 |
免费 | 1493篇 |
国内免费 | 19篇 |
专业分类
40853篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 389篇 |
2019年 | 539篇 |
2018年 | 702篇 |
2017年 | 784篇 |
2016年 | 772篇 |
2015年 | 566篇 |
2014年 | 663篇 |
2013年 | 3269篇 |
2012年 | 1152篇 |
2011年 | 1248篇 |
2010年 | 845篇 |
2009年 | 816篇 |
2008年 | 1150篇 |
2007年 | 1089篇 |
2006年 | 1012篇 |
2005年 | 879篇 |
2004年 | 833篇 |
2003年 | 853篇 |
2002年 | 854篇 |
2001年 | 1294篇 |
2000年 | 1257篇 |
1999年 | 954篇 |
1998年 | 495篇 |
1997年 | 408篇 |
1996年 | 394篇 |
1995年 | 414篇 |
1994年 | 402篇 |
1993年 | 382篇 |
1992年 | 812篇 |
1991年 | 783篇 |
1990年 | 739篇 |
1989年 | 684篇 |
1988年 | 641篇 |
1987年 | 642篇 |
1986年 | 608篇 |
1985年 | 682篇 |
1984年 | 572篇 |
1983年 | 483篇 |
1982年 | 398篇 |
1981年 | 432篇 |
1979年 | 578篇 |
1978年 | 459篇 |
1977年 | 439篇 |
1976年 | 403篇 |
1975年 | 508篇 |
1974年 | 514篇 |
1973年 | 538篇 |
1972年 | 443篇 |
1968年 | 422篇 |
1966年 | 422篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
72.
73.
74.
A comparison of two measures of egocentrism 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
L D Cohn S G Millstein C E Irwin N E Adler S M Kegeles P Dolcini G Stone 《Journal of personality assessment》1988,52(2):212-222
This study examined the relationship between two prominent measures of egocentrism, Enright's Adolescent Egocentrism Scale (AES) and Elkind's Imaginary Audience Scale (IAS), in a sample of 458 adolescents, between 10 and 15 years old. Both correlational and factor analyses indicated that the two measures assess distinctly different phenomena. As predicted, IAS scores were correlated with self-reported levels of shyness, nervousness, and social skills. These results provide partial evidence for the validity of the IAS as a measure of self-consciousness, though not necessarily egocentrism per se. The validity of the AES was not examined. 相似文献
75.
The present study is concerned with the perceptual information about the body and space underlying the act of catching a ball. In a series of four experiments, subjects were asked to catch a luminous ball under various visual conditions. In general, catching in a normally illuminated room was contrasted with catching the luminous ball in an otherwise completely dark room. In the third and fourth experiments, intermediate conditions of visual information were included. The results suggest that it is possible to catch a ball with one hand when only the ball is visible, but performance is better when the subject has the benefit of a rich visual environment and two hands. The second experiment indicated that subject performance does improve with practice in the dark, but time spent in the darkened room itself doesn't result in a significant decrement in performance. Results of the third study suggest that vision of one's hand does not aid in the performance of this task whereas the presence of a minimal visual frame appears to aid performance. The final study examined the relation between catching performance and body sway under similar visual conditions. Results of this experiment imply that persons who exhibit relatively little postural sway in full-room lighting performed better at this catching task. 相似文献
76.
J T Cacioppo J S Martzke R E Petty L G Tassinary 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1988,54(4):592-604
Previous research has demonstrated that mild negative emotional imagery and unpleasant sensory stimuli lead to greater electromyographic activity over the brow muscle region than mild positive imagery and stimuli, even in the absence of significant changes in visceral and general facial EMG activity. Previous research has not addressed whether electromyographic responses over the brow region are a sensitive and specific index of emotions, however, since a multiplicity of events lead to changes in brow activity. In this research, facial electromyographic and audiovisual recordings were obtained while individuals were interviewed about themselves. Afterwards, individuals were asked to describe what they had been thinking of during specific segments of the interview marked by distinctive electromyographic responses over the brow region in the context of ongoing but stable levels of activity elsewhere in the face. The results are interpreted in terms of a continuous flow hypothesis of affect-laden information processing. 相似文献
77.
Social contagion of binge eating 总被引:6,自引:0,他引:6
C S Crandall 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1988,55(4):588-598
A social psychological account of the acquisition of binge eating, analogous to the classic social psychological work, "Social Pressures in Informal Groups" (Festinger, Schachter, & Back, 1950), is suggested and tested in two college sororities. In these sororities, clear evidence of group norms about appropriate binge-eating behavior was found; in one sorority, the more one binged, the more popular one was. In the other, popularity was associated with binging the right amount: Those who binged too much or too little were less popular than those who binged at the mean. Evidence of social pressures to binge eat were found as well. By the end of the academic year, a sorority member's binge eating could be predicted from the binge-eating level of her friends (average r = .31). As friendship groups grew more cohesive, a sorority member's binge eating grew more and more like that of her friends (average r = .35). The parsimony of a social psychological account of the acquisition of binge eating behavior is shown. I argue that there is no great mystery to how bulimia has become such a serious problem for today's women. Binge eating seems to be an acquired pattern of behavior, perhaps through modeling, and appears to be learned much like any other set of behaviors. Like other behaviors, it is under substantial social control. 相似文献
78.
79.
F. Charles Mace Michael L. Hock Joseph S. Lalli Barbara J. West Phillip Belfiore Elizabeth Pinter D. Kirby Brown 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1988,21(2):123-141
Behavioral momentum refers to the tendency for behavior to persist following a change in environmental conditions. The greater the rate of reinforcement, the greater the behavioral momentum. The intervention for noncompliance consisted of issuing a sequence of commands with which the subject was very likely to comply (i.e., high-probability commands) immediately prior to issuing a low-probability command. In each of five experiments, the high-probability command sequence resulted in a “momentum” of compliant responding that persisted when a low-probability request was issued. Results showed the antecedent high-probability command sequence increased compliance and decreased compliance latency and task duration. “Momentum-like” effects were shown to be distinct from experimenter attention and to depend on the contiguity between the high-probability command sequence and the low-probability command. 相似文献
80.
Brenda L. Dawson D. Balfour Jeffrey James A. Walsh 《Journal of applied social psychology》1988,18(16):1353-1360
This study investigated the effect of television food commercials on children's self-control within a resistance to temptation paradigm. Commercial type, food stimulus type, and the child's sex provided the three independent variables in a 4 × 4 × 2 factorial design. Behavioral and self-report indices of temptation and control were measured. Children were significantly more tempted to transgress for the low-nutrition food, regardless of the commercial shown. Sex differences in reported degree of temptation were found. However, the television commercials did not affect the children's latency to transgress. 相似文献