全文获取类型
收费全文 | 13237篇 |
免费 | 493篇 |
国内免费 | 2篇 |
专业分类
13732篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 195篇 |
2019年 | 217篇 |
2018年 | 303篇 |
2017年 | 332篇 |
2016年 | 349篇 |
2015年 | 240篇 |
2014年 | 292篇 |
2013年 | 1320篇 |
2012年 | 500篇 |
2011年 | 476篇 |
2010年 | 323篇 |
2009年 | 328篇 |
2008年 | 435篇 |
2007年 | 435篇 |
2006年 | 390篇 |
2005年 | 352篇 |
2004年 | 361篇 |
2003年 | 315篇 |
2002年 | 364篇 |
2001年 | 253篇 |
2000年 | 209篇 |
1999年 | 217篇 |
1998年 | 174篇 |
1997年 | 165篇 |
1996年 | 157篇 |
1995年 | 165篇 |
1994年 | 141篇 |
1993年 | 152篇 |
1992年 | 178篇 |
1991年 | 187篇 |
1990年 | 155篇 |
1989年 | 147篇 |
1988年 | 141篇 |
1987年 | 132篇 |
1986年 | 128篇 |
1985年 | 157篇 |
1984年 | 171篇 |
1983年 | 172篇 |
1982年 | 170篇 |
1981年 | 185篇 |
1980年 | 142篇 |
1979年 | 188篇 |
1978年 | 190篇 |
1977年 | 175篇 |
1976年 | 151篇 |
1975年 | 180篇 |
1974年 | 174篇 |
1973年 | 133篇 |
1972年 | 120篇 |
1971年 | 103篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
82.
James C. Overholser Ph.D. 《Journal of Contemporary Psychotherapy》1987,17(4):250-269
Passive and dependent qualities are commonly found in psychiatric patients suffering from a variety of disorders. While such tendencies are often secondary to an Axis I clinical syndrome, they are occasionally more persistent and refractory. The present article attempts to provide therapeutic guidelines useful when working with clients displaying stable personality traits of passivity and dependency. An attempt is made to adapt and combine several therapeutic techniques to be applicable to passive-dependent clients. More specifically, problem-solving approaches and self-control strategies can be used to facilitate autonomy if precautions are taken to reduce the dogmatic and didactic styles commonly employed with these techniques. The Socratic method of interviewing is posited as a useful means of developing problem-solving skills without creating unnecessary dependence on the therapist. Self-control strategies can help reduce the reliance on others as the primary source of satisfaction for dependent clients. In this way, passive-dependent clients may become more self- reliant and autonomous in their daily functioning.The author would like to thank Patti Watson, Katie Brooks, and Estyn Williams for comments made on an earlier draft of this article. 相似文献
83.
84.
James Blachowicz 《Synthese》1987,71(3):235-321
In recent years, there have been some attempts to defend the legitimacy of a non-inductive generative logic of discovery whose strategy is to analyze a variety of constraints on the actual generation of explanatory hypotheses. These proposed new theories, however, are only weakly generative (relying on sophisticated processes of elimination) rather than strongly generative (embodying processes of correction).This paper develops a strongly generative theory which holds that we can come to know something new only as a variant of what we already know — and that the novelty of this variant is not thereby eliminated nor beyond our powers of characterization, a double requirement that is vital for resolving the Meno paradox. In this light, the discovery of a new hypothesis is taken as the correction of an antecedent hypothesis in response to the discrepancies between the predictions generated by that antecedent hypothesis and the desired result (e.g. the actual data to be explained). This process comprises two parallel operations: the first, which demonstrates the positive role of the facts in generating new explanations, involves a mapping between multiple hypotheses and sets of predictions generated from those hypotheses, for the purpose of taking the actual data as a determinable variant of neighboring sets of predictions. This mapping permits the facts to indicate how corrective adjustments in the working hypothesis should be made; the second operation, which demonstrates the positive role of explanations in generating new facts, involves a mapping between differently construed versions of the actual data and the conceptualizations derived from those perceptual versions, for the purpose of taking the working hypothesis as a determinable variant of these neighboring conceptualizations. This mapping permits a given hypothesis to generate predictions increasingly closer to the actual facts.The proposed theory provides the basis for a reformed conception of justification. Because hypotheses are meaningful only as variants of neighboring hypotheses, and because such variation is corrective, their justification in the reformed sense will incorporate not only their justification in the traditional sense, but their generation as well. 相似文献
85.
Kevin C. Mooney Robert Thompson James M. Nelson 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1987,15(1):67-73
The relationship between risk factors and the severity and type of childhood disorder, as measured by parent-completed Child Behavior Checklists, was examined for 768 children, ages 4–16, seen at a child mental health center. Regression analyses revealed no significant relationships between any combination of risk factors and the total number of behavior problems, internalizing, or externalizing scores for the entire group, males and females separately, or for the age and sex groupings of 6- to 11- and 12-to 16-yearolds. Chi-square tests revealed no relationship of profile type with any risk factor or with total number of risk factors. Implications for broad-based child assessment and risk factor research within a clinical population are discussed.We wish to thank Orin Bolstad, Linda Magnuson, and Julian Taplin for their assistance in this project. 相似文献
86.
William T. Riley P. Alex Mabe James M. Schear 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1987,9(1):67-74
Seventy-five patients admitted to the psychiatric unit of a general hospital were administered a brief neuropsychological screening battery. Discharge diagnoses obtained from the medical records revealed that 20% had brainbased pathology. A discriminant function analysis resulted in a five-variable model (Symbol-Digit Modalities Test, educational level, Psychiatric-Organic subscale of the MMPI, and Shipley Vocabulary and Abstraction scores) which correctly classified 84% of the subjects, with 87% correct identification of the brain-damaged group. A jackknife cross-validation procedure resulted in comparable hit rates. 相似文献
87.
88.
89.
90.
O J Harvey 《Journal of personality》1986,54(4):659-675
This study focused on the relationship of belief systems as a configural construct and conservatism-liberalism to attitudes toward the death penalty and other punishments for offenses of varying severity Extrapersonalists, the most concretely functioning of the four conceptual or belief systems posited by Harvey, Hunt, and Schroder (1961), were most in favor of the death penalty They also endorsed the most severe punishment for a variety of crimes, to a particularly greater extent than did representatives of either of the two more abstractly functioning systems The belief dimensions of Openness, Evaluativeness, and Complexity, as well as the frequency of church attendance, correlated more highly and consistently with attitudes toward punishment than did either Conservatism-Liberalism or gender Both of the latter variables failed to correlate with a number of the outcome variables and related at only low levels to the others The greater predictive power of a configural conception of personality or belief systems over a unidimensional conception seems to have been demonstrated Configural concepts may additionally be generally superior to multidimensional concepts treated linearly 相似文献