首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   75005篇
  免费   3099篇
  国内免费   48篇
  2020年   875篇
  2019年   1086篇
  2018年   1536篇
  2017年   1550篇
  2016年   1666篇
  2015年   1154篇
  2014年   1389篇
  2013年   6692篇
  2012年   2445篇
  2011年   2651篇
  2010年   1608篇
  2009年   1587篇
  2008年   2334篇
  2007年   2275篇
  2006年   2026篇
  2005年   1840篇
  2004年   1817篇
  2003年   1680篇
  2002年   1749篇
  2001年   2399篇
  2000年   2293篇
  1999年   1731篇
  1998年   950篇
  1997年   835篇
  1996年   751篇
  1995年   764篇
  1994年   704篇
  1993年   724篇
  1992年   1388篇
  1991年   1314篇
  1990年   1311篇
  1989年   1200篇
  1988年   1135篇
  1987年   1108篇
  1986年   1148篇
  1985年   1194篇
  1984年   981篇
  1983年   866篇
  1982年   693篇
  1981年   680篇
  1979年   954篇
  1978年   757篇
  1975年   857篇
  1974年   943篇
  1973年   838篇
  1972年   793篇
  1971年   681篇
  1968年   784篇
  1967年   736篇
  1966年   677篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
51.
52.
Human communication is thoroughly context bound. We present two experiments investigating the importance of the shared context, that is, the amount of knowledge two interlocutors have in common, for the successful emergence and use of novel conventions. Using a referential communication task where black‐and‐white pictorial symbols are used to convey colors, pairs of participants build shared conventions peculiar to their dyad without experimenter feedback, relying purely on ostensive‐inferential communication. Both experiments demonstrate that access to the visual context promotes more successful communication. Importantly, success improves cumulatively, supporting the view that pairs establish conventional ways of using the symbols to communicate. Furthermore, Experiment 2 suggests that dyads with access to the visual context successfully adapt the conventions built for one color space to another color space, unlike dyads lacking it. In linking experimental pragmatics with language evolution, the study illustrates the benefits of exploring the emergence of linguistic conventions using an ostensive‐inferential model of communication.  相似文献   
53.
54.
Wray  K. Brad 《Synthese》2019,196(3):819-832
Synthese - Hoyningen-Huene (Systematicity: the nature of science, Oxford University Press, Oxford, 2013) develops an account of what science is, distinguishing it from common sense. According to...  相似文献   
55.
56.
57.
58.
By roughly 6 years of age, children acquire the stereotype that men are more competent than women in science, technology, engineering, and mathematics (STEM), potentially leading to greater trust in scientific information provided by men. This study tested whether 3- to 8-year-old children differentially endorsed conflicting information about science and toys presented by male and female informants depicted as a ‘man’ and ‘woman’ (Exp1) or ‘scientists’ (Exp2). Children were expected to endorse toy testimony from gender-matched informants; thus, the key question concerned endorsement of science testimony. In Exp1 (N = 149), boys and girls showed a same-gender informant preference for toy testimony; however, girls endorsed the male informant's testimony more for science than for toys – but only when tested by a male experimenter. In Exp2 (N = 264), boys and girls showed a same-gender preference, irrespective of content. Findings suggest that STEM-related gender stereotypes might lead girls to trust scientific information presented by men over women in certain contexts.  相似文献   
59.
陈莲笙道长生于1917年10月25日,上海市人.历任中国道教协会副会长,中国道教协会顾问,上海市道教协会会长.上海市道教协会名誉会长,上海城隍庙住持,上海道学院院长,<上海道教>杂志主编,上海市宗教学会理事,上海市政协第七届委员,上海市政协第八、九届常委等职.  相似文献   
60.
In this paper, we organize past and present theories and models of creativity by using a new conceptual framework—the creativity matrix—with the aim of highlighting the dimensions of creativity we know a lot about and those we tend to either ignore or find difficult to study. This matrix is formed by bringing together a developmental model of creativity (the 4 C's) and a structural one (the 5 A's). We start by briefly describing these two conceptual frameworks, and then, we proceed to exploring the matrix itself by describing how the 5 A's are dynamically organized at each “level” of the 4 C's. Importantly, our overview of the matrix is informed by existing models and concepts that address one of more of the C's and the A's. This gives us a unique opportunity to take stock of what has been studied so far and, toward the end, consider new avenues for the development of theory and research agendas within creativity studies.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号