全文获取类型
收费全文 | 8689篇 |
免费 | 152篇 |
专业分类
8841篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 104篇 |
2019年 | 148篇 |
2018年 | 157篇 |
2017年 | 195篇 |
2016年 | 218篇 |
2015年 | 161篇 |
2014年 | 208篇 |
2013年 | 924篇 |
2012年 | 330篇 |
2011年 | 321篇 |
2010年 | 235篇 |
2009年 | 219篇 |
2008年 | 311篇 |
2007年 | 298篇 |
2006年 | 282篇 |
2005年 | 255篇 |
2004年 | 266篇 |
2003年 | 245篇 |
2002年 | 293篇 |
2001年 | 107篇 |
2000年 | 102篇 |
1999年 | 130篇 |
1998年 | 142篇 |
1997年 | 124篇 |
1996年 | 112篇 |
1995年 | 133篇 |
1994年 | 109篇 |
1993年 | 105篇 |
1992年 | 107篇 |
1991年 | 101篇 |
1990年 | 91篇 |
1989年 | 82篇 |
1988年 | 66篇 |
1987年 | 66篇 |
1986年 | 69篇 |
1985年 | 85篇 |
1984年 | 106篇 |
1983年 | 120篇 |
1982年 | 116篇 |
1981年 | 149篇 |
1980年 | 108篇 |
1979年 | 122篇 |
1978年 | 126篇 |
1977年 | 117篇 |
1976年 | 102篇 |
1975年 | 107篇 |
1974年 | 107篇 |
1973年 | 70篇 |
1972年 | 63篇 |
1969年 | 57篇 |
排序方式: 共有8841条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
181.
This study used functional magnetic resonance imaging (fMRI) to examine brain-behavior correlations in a group of 16 children (9- to 12-year-olds). Activation was measured during a semantic judgment task presented in either the visual or auditory modality that required the individual to determine whether a final word was related in meaning to one of two previous words (e.g., found-tank-lost). The main finding was that higher performers (i.e., accuracy) were associated with more activation in posterior representational systems including the inferior and middle temporal gyri, whereas lower performers were associated with more activation in anterior regions including the inferior and middle frontal gyri. This pattern of results was interpreted as reflecting an elaborated semantic representational system in temporal areas for the high accuracy performers that allowed them to efficiently and accurately make meaning based judgments. The low accuracy performers may have an inaccurate or weakly interconnected semantic system that results in greater use of frontal areas in a feature selection process. 相似文献
182.
This study compares the tendency for numerals to elicit spontaneous perceptions of colour or taste (synaesthesia) with the tendency to visualise numbers as occupying particular visuo-spatial configurations (number forms). The prevalence of number forms was found to be significantly higher in synaesthetes experiencing colour compared both to synaesthetes experiencing taste and to control participants lacking any synaesthetic experience. This suggests that the presence of synaesthetic colour sensations enhances the tendency to explicitly represent numbers in a visuo-spatial format although the two symptoms may nevertheless be logically independent (i.e. it is possible to have number forms without colour, and coloured numbers without forms). Number forms are equally common in men and women, unlike previous reports of synaesthesia that have suggested a strong female bias. Individuals who possess a number form are also likely to possess visuo-spatial forms for other ordinal sequences (e.g. days, months, letters) which suggests that it is the ordinal nature of numbers rather than numerical quantity that gives rise to this particular mode of representation. Finally, we also describe some consequences of number forms for performance in a number comparison task. 相似文献
183.
Woodworth (1938) reported that naming latency increased linearly with the number of digits per number (number length). In the present study, the Sternberg memory scanning paradigm was utilized to investigate this effect. It was found that the slope of the memory scanning function increased linearly with number length: memory scanning time was 40 msec for one-digit numbers, 70 msec for two-digit numbers and 101 msec for 3 digit numbers. The intercepts of the memory scanning functions did not differ for the three types of numbers. Thus the increase in latency may be due to the memory comparison stage of processing. The data suggest that a memory comparison operation occurs for each digit position of the complex memory items composed of more than one digit. 相似文献
184.
James Jaccard Daniel Hand Lucille Ku Kathleen Richardson Rudolfo Abella 《Journal of applied social psychology》1981,11(3):181-191
Four factors (effectiveness, health risks, cost, and convenience) were orthogonally manipulated in a 3 × 2 × 2 × 2 factorial design to determine their relative importance in influencing attitudes toward and intentions to use male oral contraceptives. The experimental design also permitted a test of the assumption of additivity underlying expectancy-value models of attitude and the assumption of absence of context effects. Results were consistent with expectancy-value models of the relationship between beliefs and attitudes. In addition, it was found that health risks and effedtiveness, in that order, were the most important factors influencing receptivity to male oral contraceptives, with the effects of cost and convenience being mediated by these two factors. Sex differences in attitudes toward male oral contraceptives were also observed. 相似文献
185.
186.
James Hunter M.S.W. 《Journal of religion and health》1981,20(2):124-132
The importance of a counselor's philosophical point of departure is emphasized. Viewing the self as a conscious, free, and responsible decision-making actor in a world of substantial and self-transcendent meanings and purposes is suggested as the core of a definition that uses an existentialist point of departure. In the light of this definition, four specific inadequacies of counseling that uses a natural-scientific point of departure are identified and discussed: 1) it is overreductionistic; 2) it reinforces the view of self as helpless victim; 3) it disregards the importance of transpersonal concerns; 4) it ignores the importance of conflicting world views between therapist and client. 相似文献
187.
James E. Patton Donald K. Routh Stuart I. Offenbach 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》1981,9(3):355-370
Three experiments examined the influence of videotaped classroom events on the academic performance and study behavior of reading-disabled and normally reading children in grades 1 through 6. In experiments 1 and 2a an experimenter-controlled presentation of these distractors resulted in performance decrements, the magnitude of which was greater for higher difficulty tasks. The distractor effects were similar for the two groups in this condition. However, when the children themselves were allowed to control their degree of exposure to the same distractors in experiment 2b, the reading-disabled children were less likely to act to escape the distraction. Results are discussed in terms of children's metacognitive awareness of distraction, and implications for the classroom are considered. 相似文献
188.
189.
Hamilton JW 《Psychoanalytic review》2002,89(6):889-909
190.
James D. Johnson Carolyn H. Simmons Amanda Jordav Leslie Maclean Jeffrey Taddei Duane Thomas John F. Dovidio William Reed 《Journal of applied social psychology》2002,32(6):1208-1223
White university students participated in a study to investigate the impact of defendant race and empathetic induction on a subsequent juror decision-making task. Participants read a passage involving a Black or a White defendant in a criminal case. They were subsequently induced to feel no empathy, low empathy, or high empathy for the defendant. When compared to participants in the low- and control empathy conditions, those in the high-empathy condition reported greater target empathy, made attributions that were more situational, and assigned more lenient punishments. The results also indicate that group membership can moderate the impact of empathetic induction. When compared to the participants in the Black defendant condition, those in the White defendant condition reported greater target empathy, made attributions that were more situational, and assigned more lenient punishments. Implications for both empathy and judicial decision-making research are discussed. 相似文献