全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4298篇 |
免费 | 54篇 |
专业分类
4352篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 35篇 |
2023年 | 44篇 |
2022年 | 44篇 |
2021年 | 80篇 |
2020年 | 86篇 |
2019年 | 124篇 |
2018年 | 153篇 |
2017年 | 157篇 |
2016年 | 175篇 |
2015年 | 125篇 |
2014年 | 146篇 |
2013年 | 512篇 |
2012年 | 214篇 |
2011年 | 251篇 |
2010年 | 146篇 |
2009年 | 147篇 |
2008年 | 203篇 |
2007年 | 162篇 |
2006年 | 146篇 |
2005年 | 112篇 |
2004年 | 132篇 |
2003年 | 140篇 |
2002年 | 105篇 |
2001年 | 81篇 |
2000年 | 87篇 |
1999年 | 61篇 |
1998年 | 48篇 |
1997年 | 40篇 |
1996年 | 24篇 |
1995年 | 28篇 |
1994年 | 27篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 39篇 |
1991年 | 27篇 |
1990年 | 49篇 |
1989年 | 49篇 |
1988年 | 39篇 |
1987年 | 23篇 |
1986年 | 30篇 |
1985年 | 31篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 22篇 |
1982年 | 18篇 |
1981年 | 11篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 15篇 |
1978年 | 14篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1972年 | 12篇 |
1967年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有4352条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
881.
882.
Jorg Huijding Andy P. Field Katrien Vandenbosch Machteld van Oeveren 《Behaviour research and therapy》2009,47(6):471-477
We examined the effects of training to approach or avoid novel animals on fear-related responses in children. Ninety-five primary school children (9-13 years old) were instructed to repeatedly push away or pull closer pictures of novel animals. We tested whether this manipulation would lead to changes in self-reported attitudes, implicit attitudes, fear beliefs, and avoidance behaviors towards these animals.The training produced more positive self-reported attitudes towards the pulled animal and more negative attitudes towards the pushed animal. After the training, girls reported more fear and avoidance of the pushed animal than of the pulled animal, while such training effects were absent in boys. No significant training effects were observed on implicit attitudes. Interestingly, the level of anxiety disorder symptoms prior to training was related to some of the training effects: Stronger prior fear was related to stronger changes in self-reported attitudes, and in boys, also to fear beliefs.The finding that a simple approach-avoidance training influences children's fear-related responses lends support to general theories of fear acquisition in children as well as to models that try to explain the intergenerational transmission of anxiety. [184 words] 相似文献
883.
Iris M. Engelhard Peter J. de Jong Mark van Overveld 《Behaviour research and therapy》2009,47(10):887-892
Anxious individuals tend to overestimate the probability that encounters with anxiogenic stimuli (CS) will be followed by aversive consequences (UCS). This study examined whether such (biased) UCS expectancies predict the persistence of PTSD symptoms. A total of 265 soldiers were recruited before a four-month deployment to Iraq in 2004. About 2-5 months after deployment, 171 (65%) soldiers completed self-report scales about adverse events in Iraq and PTSD symptoms, and a UCS expectancy task. In this task, participants were exposed to a series of deployment-related and deployment-unrelated (control) picture stimuli. For each trial, the participants indicated the subjective probability that a particular slide would be followed by an imminent loud noise. Around 15 months after deployment, 130 (76%) soldiers completed surveys about PTSD symptoms again. Only a small group of participants had high levels of PTSD symptoms. Regression analyses showed that the level of PTSD symptoms at 15 months was predicted by earlier PTSD symptoms, but also and independently by an enhanced UCS expectancy to deployment-related stimuli. The findings support the notion that UCS expectancy bias contributes to the persistence of PTSD symptoms. 相似文献
884.
In one of the essays in his recent book on Christianity, La déclosion (2005), Nancy discusses the relationship between Judaism and Christianity. Nancy opens this discussion with a reference to
Lyotard’s book on this relationship: Un trait d’union (1993). Both Lyotard and Nancy examine a very early figure in the emergence of Christianity from Judaism—whereas Lyotard
focuses on the epistles of Paul, Nancy reads the epistle of James. Lyotard concludes that the hyphen in the expression ‘Judeo-Christian’
actually conceals ‘the most impenetrable abyss within Western thought’. With this abyss, Lyotard refers to the point of departure
of Judaism: the event in which a Voice has left behind letters, inaugurating an interminable work of interpretation. For Nancy,
however, it is rather Christianity, and therefore, Western culture, which is deconstructive in nature. Its composition is
co-original with a decomposition, and therefore, with an openness. In James, Nancy finds an emphasis on praxis, in such a
way that existence is to be understood as transcendent within itself. With this reading of James, Nancy seems to deny that
there is a fundamental difference between Judaism and Christianity. In order to clarify the differences between Lyotard and
Nancy, it is shown that, in Lyotard’s view, an unsublatable alterity comes with aisthèsis, whereas in Nancy’s view, alterity comes with existence as such. 相似文献
885.
886.
L. van Domburgh R. Loeber D. Bezemer R. Stallings M. Stouthamer-Loeber 《Journal of abnormal child psychology》2009,37(7):967-980
Childhood predictors of adolescent offending careers were studied in 310 boys from the longitudinal Pittsburgh Youth Study
who started offending prior to age 12. Three main groups were distinguished: serious persisters (n = 95), moderately serious persisters (n = 117), desisters (n = 63), and an intermittent group (n = 35). Group membership was predicted using risk and promotive factors measured in childhood. Serious and moderately serious
persisters could be distinguished well from desisters (29.2% and 32.3% explained variance). Distinction between the two persister
groups proved somewhat more difficult (20.9% explained variance). More serious persisters than desisters showed disruptive
behavior, while moderately serious persisters fell in between. Further, more moderately serious persisters were marked by
social disadvantage. Family involvement, small family and positive peer relationships were promotive of desistance. Concluding,
early onset offenders show considerable heterogeneity in their adolescent offending careers which seem to some extent to be
predicted by different sets of risk and promotive factors. 相似文献
887.
888.
van Linden S Vroomen J 《Journal of experimental psychology. Human perception and performance》2007,33(6):1483-1494
Listeners hearing an ambiguous phoneme flexibly adjust their phonetic categories in accordance with information telling what the phoneme should be (i.e., recalibration). Here the authors compared recalibration induced by lipread versus lexical information. Listeners were exposed to an ambiguous phoneme halfway between /t/ and /p/ dubbed onto a face articulating /t/ or /p/ or embedded in a Dutch word ending in /t/ (e.g., groot [big]) or /p/ (knoop [button]). In a posttest, participants then categorized auditory tokens as /t/ or /p/. Lipread and lexical aftereffects were comparable in size (Experiment 1), dissipated about equally fast (Experiment 2), were enhanced by exposure to a contrast phoneme (Experiment 3), and were not affected by a 3-min silence interval (Experiment 4). Exposing participants to 1 instead of both phoneme categories did not make the phenomenon more robust (Experiment 5). Despite the difference in nature (bottom-up vs. top-down information), lipread and lexical information thus appear to serve a similar role in phonetic adjustments. 相似文献
889.
Ramsey G Bastian ML van Schaik C 《The Behavioral and brain sciences》2007,30(4):393-407; discussion 407-32
Innovation is a key component of most definitions of culture and intelligence. Additionally, innovations may affect a species' ecology and evolution. Nonetheless, conceptual and empirical work on innovation has only recently begun. In particular, largely because the existing operational definition (first occurrence in a population) requires long-term studies of populations, there has been no systematic study of innovation in wild animals. To facilitate such study, we have produced a new definition of innovation: Innovation is the process that generates in an individual a novel learned behavior that is not simply a consequence of social learning or environmental induction. Using this definition, we propose a new operational approach for distinguishing innovations in the field. The operational criteria employ information from the following sources: (1) the behavior's geographic and local prevalence and individual frequency; (2) properties of the behavior, such as the social role of the behavior, the context in which the behavior is exhibited, and its similarity to other behaviors; (3) changes in the occurrence of the behavior over time; and (4) knowledge of spontaneous or experimentally induced behavior in captivity. These criteria do not require long-term studies at a single site, but information from multiple populations of a species will generally be needed. These criteria are systematized into a dichotomous key that can be used to assess whether a behavior observed in the field is likely to be an innovation. 相似文献
890.
Jacoba M. Spikman Henk A. L. Kiers Betto G. Deelman Adriaan H. van Zomeren 《Brain and cognition》2001,47(3):446-460
The present study aimed to examine the construct validity of three aspects of attention, namely focused, divided, and supervisory control of attention. Factor-analytic techniques were applied to scores of healthy subjects on a series of neuropsychological tests tapping these aspects of attention. The two components found did not match the hypothesized aspects and were labeled as Memory-driven Action and Stimulus-driven Reaction. The second question was whether the same components could be found in a group of patients with CHI. The pattern of attentional functions found in healthy subjects had changed qualitatively in patients with CHI. A possible explanation for this result in terms of a shift from automatic to controlled processing is discussed. 相似文献