首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   23866篇
  免费   193篇
  国内免费   1篇
  24060篇
  2024年   14篇
  2023年   56篇
  2022年   35篇
  2021年   70篇
  2020年   86篇
  2019年   131篇
  2018年   3586篇
  2017年   2931篇
  2016年   2377篇
  2015年   309篇
  2014年   188篇
  2013年   483篇
  2012年   777篇
  2011年   2567篇
  2010年   2608篇
  2009年   1559篇
  2008年   1850篇
  2007年   2309篇
  2006年   133篇
  2005年   351篇
  2004年   296篇
  2003年   231篇
  2002年   158篇
  2001年   91篇
  2000年   120篇
  1999年   60篇
  1998年   58篇
  1997年   44篇
  1996年   39篇
  1995年   27篇
  1994年   15篇
  1993年   30篇
  1992年   30篇
  1991年   38篇
  1990年   46篇
  1989年   21篇
  1988年   34篇
  1987年   24篇
  1986年   27篇
  1985年   25篇
  1984年   29篇
  1983年   25篇
  1982年   11篇
  1979年   15篇
  1978年   15篇
  1977年   20篇
  1976年   9篇
  1974年   10篇
  1973年   8篇
  1971年   9篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
Jan de Leeuw 《Psychometrika》1977,42(1):141-144
Kruskal has proposed two modifications of monotone regression that can be applied if there are ties in nonmetric scaling data. In this note we prove Kruskal's conjecture that his algorithms give the optimal least squares solution of these modified monotone regression problems. We also propose another (third) approach for dealing with ties.Comments by Dr. J. B. Kruskal have been most helpful.  相似文献   
82.
83.
84.
85.
86.
Obsessional-compulsive imagery   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
  相似文献   
87.
An observant cognitive therapy patient reported that the first dysfunctional thought elicited and treated in homework sessions was more resistant to change than later distorted thoughts. To determine whether this was due to the fact that this thought was elicited first, or to the fact that it was treated first, systematic data were collected from 9 patients in 48 sessions of cognitive therapy. In half of these sessions, dysfunctional thoughts were treated in the order they were elicited; in half of the sessions, they were treated in the reverse order. Results showed that the first dysfunctional thought elicited changed least; there was no effect of order of treatment. Several possible explanations of this finding are offered.  相似文献   
88.
Food deprived, heterogeneous strain (HS/IBG) mice were trained on two different discrimination tasks for food reinforcement. In one experiment animals were trained to make spatial discriminations in a T maze. Immediately after training they were given subcutaneous injections of either substance P (1 ng/g) or vehicle. Twenty-four hours later the animals were given reversal training in the same maze. The results showed that substance P-treated animals took significantly longer to acquire the reversal habit than did control mice. In a second experiment, animals were trained to make visual discriminations in a T maze. Immediately after reaching acquisition criterion animals were injected with either substance P (1 ng/g) or vehicle. Different groups of mice were retrained on the same task either 1, 2, 3, or 7 days after original learning. Savings scores were calculated and, at every interval, substance P-treated mice retained the task better than control animals. One interpretation of these data is that substance P-treated mice remembered the original task significantly better than vehicle-injected control animals.  相似文献   
89.
大脑两半球功能不对称性与儿童学业成绩   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
本研究调查了 3 1 8名小学一至三年级学生大脑两半球优势情况 ,然后对不同优势半球学生的语文和数学成绩进行了比较。结果发现优势半球为右半球的学生人数较少 ,只有 3 .8% ,明显少于优势半球为左半球的学生 (χ2 =2 3 9.83 0 ,p<0 .0 0 1 )。对不同优势半球学生的语文和数学成绩的比较 ,未发现两者有显著差异 (语文 :F=1 .1 3 0 ,p>0 .0 5 ;数学 :F=0 .5 75 ,p>0 .0 5 )。  相似文献   
90.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号