全文获取类型
收费全文 | 417篇 |
免费 | 6篇 |
专业分类
423篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 3篇 |
2019年 | 8篇 |
2018年 | 11篇 |
2017年 | 14篇 |
2016年 | 12篇 |
2015年 | 10篇 |
2014年 | 5篇 |
2013年 | 52篇 |
2012年 | 13篇 |
2011年 | 20篇 |
2010年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 11篇 |
2008年 | 19篇 |
2007年 | 14篇 |
2006年 | 13篇 |
2005年 | 14篇 |
2004年 | 6篇 |
2003年 | 9篇 |
2002年 | 15篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 19篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 12篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 4篇 |
1986年 | 5篇 |
1985年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1983年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1980年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 3篇 |
1969年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 2篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有423条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
221.
222.
Abstract: Locke scholars continue to disagree over how he analyzed natural laws, real essence-power relations in physical substances. Some say he regarded them as emanations, necessitated by the corpuscular structure of real essences; for others his laws are adventitious, imposed on substances by God and contingent on divine alterable will. The second view has been increasingly favored in recent years, assisted no doubt by Edwin McCann's potent case for it in Lockean Mechanism (1985). The present article, whose authors are sympathetic to the necessitarian reading of Locke, argues against McCann's exegesis. 相似文献
223.
Alan Randich W. J. Jacobs Vincent M. Lolordo James R. Sutterer 《Quarterly journal of experimental psychology (2006)》1978,30(1):141-150
In Experiment I, groups of rats were trained to press a lever for food reinforcement on differential reinforcement of low rate (DRL) schedules which differed in parameter value. A stimulus which terminated with either a 0.5-mA or 2.0-mA electric shock was then superimposed upon each DRL baseline. In general, the magnitude of conditioned suppression was an inverse function of DRL schedule parameter and a direct function of shock intensity. Experiment II demonstrated that the rate of responding maintained by the DRL component of a multiple DRL-extinction schedule decreased during a stimulus preceding a 0.5-mA shock, whereas the rate of responding maintained by the DRL component of a multiple DRL-variable interval schedule showed little change or increased slightly during a stimulus preceding a 0.5-mA shock. 相似文献
224.
Vision in the prairie dog: spectral sensitivity and color vision 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
225.
226.
Learning to perceive the relative mass of colliding balls: the effects of ratio scaling and feedback
The theory of direct perception holds that competent observers are able to detect optical patterns that specify the relative mass of colliding balls. Heuristic theorists, on the other hand, claim that judgments of relative mass are based on variables that do not specify relative mass. We contrasted these views with an experiment in which participants were given feedback on their ratio-scaled estimates of the relative mass of simulated colliding balls. Correlations between judged relative mass and various kinetic and kinematic measures of the collisions revealed that (1) judgments of relative mass become more accurate with feedback, (2) different observers use different variables, (3) during training, many observers change which variables they use, (4) before training, observers tend to use nonspecifying variables or combinations thereof, (5) after a minimal amount of training, at least some observers seem to detect mass-specifying information, and (6) the judgments do not support a generalization of the heuristic model of Gilden and Proffitt (1989, 1994). These findings suggest that direct perception of relative mass is a skill that can be developed through appropriate training. 相似文献
227.
Kahana MJ Jacobs J 《Journal of experimental psychology. Learning, memory, and cognition》2000,26(5):1188-1197
The authors examined the effects of intraserial repetition on multitrial serial learning of random consonant lists, analyzing both learning rates and perfect trial interresponse times (IRTs). Lists varied along 3 dimensions: list length, presence or absence of a repeated element, and lag between repeated elements. After achieving a forward-recall criterion on a given list, participants (N = 20) attempted backward recall. At small lags, IRTs between the repeated elements were very short (compared with IRTs from identical positions in nonrepetition lists). At larger lags, the IRT to recall the second repeated item was substantially longer than in control lists. These results reveal a latency analogue of the Ranschburg pattern seen in accuracy data. A Ranschburg pattern was also found in participants' learning rates. These results both generalize the Ranschburg phenomenon and present further challenges to theories of serial order memory. 相似文献
228.
Jonathan Jacobs 《The Journal of value inquiry》1985,19(3):171-182
229.
Using four vignettes to study attitudes toward religious persecution, a woman or man aged 22 or 68 came to the US with no supporting documentation. A General Linear Model analysed the 12 items for each vignette. Three items were significant among the vignettes: “All immigration policies and laws should be observed even if it means that M may not be granted permission to stay”; “M’s gender might be an indicator that M wishes to do harm to the US and its citizens” and “M should be deported because s/he entered the country illegally.” Gender and age impact these perceptions. 相似文献
230.
Ruoyang Hu;Robert A. Jacobs; 《Cognitive Science》2024,48(9):e13498
Visual working memory (VWM) refers to the temporary storage and manipulation of visual information. Although visually different, objects we view and remember can share the same higher-level category information, such as an apple, orange, and banana all being classified as fruit. We study the influence of category information on VWM, focusing on the question of whether stimulus category coherence (i.e., whether all to-be-remembered items belong to the same semantic category) influences VWM performance. This question is addressed in two behavioral experiments using a change-detection paradigm and a rational analysis using an ideal observer based on a Bayesian model. Both experimental participants and the ideal observer often, but not always, performed numerically better on coherent trials (i.e., when all stimuli belonged to the same category). We hypothesize that the influence of category coherence information on VWM may be task-dependent and/or stimulus-dependent. In conditions when category coherence information is highly valuable for task performance, as indicated by the ideal observer, then participants tended to make use of it. However, when the ideal observer suggested this information was not crucial to performance, participants did not. In addition, both participants and the ideal observer showed a bias toward responding “same,” and often showed a stronger influence of category coherence on change trials. The consistencies between participant and ideal observer responses suggest participants often behaved as they did because these behaviors are optimal (or approximately so) for maximizing task performance. This may help explain conflicting results reported in the scientific literature. 相似文献