首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   7757篇
  免费   315篇
  国内免费   3篇
  2023年   48篇
  2021年   64篇
  2020年   129篇
  2019年   155篇
  2018年   187篇
  2017年   209篇
  2016年   209篇
  2015年   165篇
  2014年   202篇
  2013年   938篇
  2012年   327篇
  2011年   321篇
  2010年   198篇
  2009年   231篇
  2008年   318篇
  2007年   300篇
  2006年   273篇
  2005年   283篇
  2004年   261篇
  2003年   245篇
  2002年   246篇
  2001年   116篇
  2000年   104篇
  1999年   114篇
  1998年   127篇
  1997年   86篇
  1996年   88篇
  1995年   98篇
  1994年   82篇
  1993年   73篇
  1992年   76篇
  1991年   81篇
  1990年   61篇
  1989年   73篇
  1988年   69篇
  1987年   57篇
  1986年   57篇
  1985年   78篇
  1984年   82篇
  1983年   72篇
  1982年   63篇
  1981年   68篇
  1980年   66篇
  1979年   47篇
  1978年   68篇
  1977年   61篇
  1976年   64篇
  1975年   61篇
  1974年   57篇
  1973年   46篇
排序方式: 共有8075条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
992.
The authors sought to examine whether rumination about psychologically painful, though nontraumatic, interpersonal transgressions is associated with increased salivary cortisol. They measured salivary cortisol, rumination about a transgression, fear and anger regarding the transgressor, perceived painfulness of the transgression, and positive and negative mood in 115 undergraduates who had experienced an interpersonal transgression during the previous 7 days. They obtained measurements on as many as 5 occasions separated by approximately 14 days each. On occasions when participants reported that they had been ruminating to a degree that was greater than was typical for them, they had higher levels of salivary cortisol than was typical for them. The rumination- cortisol association appeared to be mediated by fear of the transgressor. Rumination about even moderately painful but nontraumatic life events and associated emotions are related to biological changes that may subserve social goals such as avoiding social threats. Items from the rumination scale are appended.  相似文献   
993.
994.
995.
Paul Lorenzen 《Synthese》1956,10(1):181-186
Ohne Zusammenfassung  相似文献   
996.
How do people come up with humorous ideas? In creative cognition research, exposure to good examples sometimes causes fixation (people get “stuck” on the examples) but other times sparks inspiration (people's responses are more creative). The present research examined the effects of funny and unfunny examples on joke production. A sample of 175 adults read scenarios that they completed with funny responses. All participants were instructed to be funny, but before responding they read (a) funny responses as examples of good responses to emulate, (b) unfunny responses as examples of poor responses to avoid, or (c) no examples. The participants’ own responses were rated for funniness and for similarity to the example responses, and response times were recorded. Reading either funny or unfunny examples, compared to no examples, caused people to come up with funnier jokes. Similarity to the examples was low in all conditions, so fixation was relatively modest, but people who saw unfunny examples spent more time coming up with their responses. Taken together, the findings support the growing literature showing that examples are often inspiring rather than constraining, and they imply that good and bad examples spark creative thought via different paths.  相似文献   
997.
The study investigated perceived religious discrimination and three facets of Muslim identity (psychological, behavioural, and visible) as predictors of psychological well‐being (life satisfaction and psychological symptoms) of 153 Muslim women in New Zealand. The results indicated that although visibility (wearing hijab) was associated with greater perceived discrimination, it predicted positive psychological outcomes. Analysis further revealed that the psychological (pride, belongingness, and centrality) and behavioural (engaging in Islamic practices) facets of Muslim identity moderated the relationship between perceived religious discrimination and well‐being. A strong psychological affiliation with Islam exacerbated the negative relationship between perceived religious discrimination and well‐being. Conversely, engaging in Islamic practices buffered the negative impact of discrimination. The research highlights the complexity of Muslim identity in diasporic women.  相似文献   
998.
999.
1000.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号