全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2220篇 |
免费 | 32篇 |
专业分类
2252篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 15篇 |
2022年 | 11篇 |
2021年 | 20篇 |
2020年 | 40篇 |
2019年 | 50篇 |
2018年 | 53篇 |
2017年 | 71篇 |
2016年 | 76篇 |
2015年 | 57篇 |
2014年 | 80篇 |
2013年 | 250篇 |
2012年 | 105篇 |
2011年 | 118篇 |
2010年 | 78篇 |
2009年 | 69篇 |
2008年 | 95篇 |
2007年 | 110篇 |
2006年 | 86篇 |
2005年 | 71篇 |
2004年 | 75篇 |
2003年 | 57篇 |
2002年 | 54篇 |
2001年 | 37篇 |
2000年 | 29篇 |
1999年 | 37篇 |
1998年 | 34篇 |
1997年 | 21篇 |
1996年 | 27篇 |
1995年 | 10篇 |
1994年 | 17篇 |
1993年 | 16篇 |
1992年 | 25篇 |
1991年 | 20篇 |
1990年 | 19篇 |
1989年 | 17篇 |
1988年 | 18篇 |
1987年 | 15篇 |
1986年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 19篇 |
1984年 | 20篇 |
1983年 | 18篇 |
1982年 | 14篇 |
1980年 | 13篇 |
1979年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 10篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
1974年 | 17篇 |
1973年 | 12篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1969年 | 13篇 |
排序方式: 共有2252条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
121.
Confirmatory factor analysis was used to test the invariance of an oppositional defiant disorder toward adults, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-hyperactivity/impulsivity, attention-deficit/hyperactivity disorder-inattention, and an Academic Competence factor model between mothers' and fathers' ratings within Brazilian (n = 894), Thai (n = 2,075), and American (n = 817) children with the Child and Adolescent Disruptive Behavior Inventory (G. L. Burns, T. Taylor, & J. Rusby, 2001a, 2001b). The results showed invariance of item loadings, intercepts, and residuals, as well as factor variances, covariances, and means between mothers' and fathers' ratings within each sample. Convergent and discriminant validity was also observed for the between-parent factor correlations, thus providing additional support for the construct validity of the Child and Adolescent Disruptive Behavior Inventory. The confirmatory factor analysis invariance procedure provides a much better way to examine between-source ratings of behavior problems in children than do the simple correlation and raw discrepancy score procedures. 相似文献
122.
123.
124.
An experiment is reported which demonstrates that the features used to detect a character in arrays of similar forms are a function of the stimulus context in which the target is embedded. With the use of blocked and randomised design it was shown that prior knowledge of the background aided search when the feature which was relevant for one backgroind was irrelevant for another, but no significant effect of prior knowledge was obtained when the same feature was useful for the backgrounds. 相似文献
125.
Gerald Gregory Jackson 《Journal of Employment Counseling》1972,9(3):130-139
Observation of the schism between employers and disadvantaged females, leads to the preparation of counselees for job interviews and exploration of the world of work with them. By chance, roleplaying a job interview was used during a group guidance session with Job Corps females, and the comparative success encountered with the technique encouraged this investigation. As a technique, it was found to be flexible, adaptable, and timesaving. It was of benefit to the counselor in six areas, and students indicated that they found the technique of value as a means of preparation for a real job interview, acquainting them with the scope of their communications problems and reducing these shortcomings. 相似文献
126.
Chris M. Herdman 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1996,49(4):1044-1061
The advantage of naming pseudohomophones over non-pseudohomophones has been interpreted as reflecting the contribution of whole-word lexical representations in phonological coding. A whole-word interpretation was further supported by Taft and Russell (1992), who reported a pseudohomophone frequency effect such that pseudohomophones were named faster if they corresponded to high- than to low-frequency base-words (e.g. poast vs. hoast ). Experiment 1 replicated this pseudohomophone frequency effect using the Taft and Russell items. Further analyses showed, however, that the pseudohomophones in Taft and Russell's high-frequency group were more orthographically similar to words than the pseudohomophones in the low-frequency group. These differences in orthography may have been the cause of the 'frequency' effects. In Experiment 2, a new set of high- and low-frequency pseudohomophones was constructed that were matched on orthographic factors (i.e. SPBF and N). With these items, a standard pseudohomophone advantage was found such that pseudohomophones were named faster and more accurately than non-pseudohomophones. However, in contrast to Taft and Russell's results, pseudohomophone naming was not related to base-word frequency. We conclude that the pseudohomophone advantage occurs at a postlexical stage in non-word naming. 相似文献
127.
Philosophia - According to Duncan Pritchard’s Philosophical Issues, 21(1), 434–455, (2011, 2012, 2015)version of epistemological disjunctivism, in paradigm cases of perceptual... 相似文献
128.
People reliably and automatically make personality inferences from facial appearance despite little evidence for their accuracy. Although such inferences are highly inter-correlated, research has traditionally focused on studying specific traits such as trustworthiness. We advocate an alternative, data-driven approach to identify and model the structure of face evaluation. Initial findings indicate that specific trait inferences can be represented within a 2D space defined by valence/trustworthiness and power/dominance evaluation of faces. Inferences along these dimensions are based on similarity to expressions signaling approach or avoidance behavior and features signaling physical strength, respectively, indicating that trait inferences from faces originate in functionally adaptive mechanisms. We conclude with a discussion of the potential role of the amygdala in face evaluation. 相似文献
129.
130.