全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1249篇 |
免费 | 16篇 |
专业分类
1265篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 19篇 |
2018年 | 20篇 |
2017年 | 30篇 |
2016年 | 35篇 |
2015年 | 12篇 |
2014年 | 20篇 |
2013年 | 149篇 |
2012年 | 42篇 |
2011年 | 48篇 |
2010年 | 43篇 |
2009年 | 20篇 |
2008年 | 44篇 |
2007年 | 42篇 |
2006年 | 47篇 |
2005年 | 38篇 |
2004年 | 52篇 |
2003年 | 31篇 |
2002年 | 36篇 |
2001年 | 16篇 |
2000年 | 16篇 |
1998年 | 13篇 |
1997年 | 14篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 9篇 |
1994年 | 12篇 |
1993年 | 15篇 |
1992年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 12篇 |
1989年 | 13篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 13篇 |
1986年 | 12篇 |
1985年 | 16篇 |
1984年 | 18篇 |
1983年 | 12篇 |
1982年 | 18篇 |
1981年 | 19篇 |
1980年 | 19篇 |
1979年 | 17篇 |
1978年 | 16篇 |
1977年 | 12篇 |
1976年 | 17篇 |
1975年 | 17篇 |
1974年 | 20篇 |
1973年 | 10篇 |
1971年 | 9篇 |
1970年 | 9篇 |
1967年 | 9篇 |
排序方式: 共有1265条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
901.
902.
Tsai CW 《Cyberpsychology, behavior and social networking》2011,14(9):505-510
In many countries, undergraduates are required to take at least one introductory computer course to enhance their computer literacy and computing skills. However, the application software education in Taiwan can hardly be deemed as effective in developing students' practical computing skills. The author applied online self-regulated learning (SRL) and collaborative learning (CL) with initiation in a blended computing course and examined the effects of different combinations on enhancing students' computing skills. Four classes, comprising 221 students, participated in this study. The online SRL and CL with initiation (G1, n?=?53), online CL with initiation (G2, n?=?68), and online CL without initiation (G3, n?=?68) were experimental groups, and the last class, receiving traditional lecture (G4, n?=?32), was the control group. The results of this study show that students who received the intervention of online SRL and CL with initiation attained significantly best grades for practical computing skills, whereas those that received the traditional lectures had statistically poorest grades among the four classes. The implications for schools and educators who plan to provide online or blended learning for their students, particularly in computing courses, are also provided in this study. 相似文献
903.
Jack Bilmes 《Human Studies》2011,34(2):129-153
This paper puts forward an argument for a systematic, technical approach to formulation in verbal interaction. I see this
as a kind of expansion of Sacks’ membership categorization analysis, and as something that is not offered (at least not in
a fully developed form) by sequential analysis, the currently dominant form of conversation analysis. In particular, I suggest
a technique for the study of “occasioned semantics,” that is, the study of structures of meaningful expressions in actual
occasions of conversation. I propose that meaning and rhetoric be approached through consideration of various dimensions or
operations or properties, including, but not limited to, contrast and co-categorization, generalization and specification,
scaling, and marking. As illustration, I consider a variety of cases, focused on generalization and specification. The paper
can be seen as a return to some classical concerns with meaning, as illuminated by more recent insights into indexicality,
social action, and interaction in recorded talk. 相似文献
904.
Jack Danielian 《American journal of psychoanalysis》2011,71(2):182-184
905.
Patients with behavioral variant frontotemporal dementia (bvFTD) can make false statements consistent with delusions or confabulations. It is unclear whether bvFTD is primarily associated with either delusions or with confabulations and whether they can be explained by the pathophysiology of this disease. In order to clarify this, we retrospectively surveyed the records of 48 patients with bvFTD for the presence of any false reports and identified four patients. Their false reports included continued interaction with a favorite but dead relation, fictitious marriages with movie stars, and two who claimed that their partner was having an affair. When confronted with the falsity of their statements, the patients conveyed a lack of certainty regarding their external or internal source but persisted in the constancy of their reports. On functional neuroimaging, the patients had predominant frontal involvement. This report found that patients with bvFTD can have both fantastic, wish fulfilling confabulations and typical content-specific delusions. We propose that both phenomena result from known disturbances of ventromedial prefrontal cortex in bvFTD, including deficits in source monitoring and in activating an automatic "doubt tag" for false reports. 相似文献
906.
Jack D. Simons Brian Hutchison Zuzana Baštecká 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2012,90(2):233-237
This article reviews the history and current status of counseling in the Czech Republic. Recommendations for advancement of the profession in a postcommunist era are offered, including the incorporation of social justice principals for the benefit of Gypsies and immigrants, collaboration between Czech and non‐Czech counselors, and counseling research with the Czech people. 相似文献
907.
Jack M. Loomis Roberta L. Klatzky Brendan McHugh Nicholas A. Giudice 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2012,74(6):1260-1267
Spatial working memory can maintain representations from vision, hearing, and touch, representations referred to here as spatial images. The present experiment addressed whether spatial images from vision and hearing that are simultaneously present within working memory retain modality-specific tags or are amodal. Observers were presented with short sequences of targets varying in angular direction, with the targets in a given sequence being all auditory, all visual, or a sequential mixture of the two. On two thirds of the trials, one of the locations was repeated, and observers had to respond as quickly as possible when detecting this repetition. Ancillary detection and localization tasks confirmed that the visual and auditory targets were perceptually comparable. Response latencies in the working memory task showed small but reliable costs in performance on trials involving a sequential mixture of auditory and visual targets, as compared with trials of pure vision or pure audition. These deficits were statistically reliable only for trials on which the modalities of the matching location switched from the penultimate to the final target in the sequence, indicating a switching cost. The switching cost for the pair in immediate succession means that the spatial images representing the target locations retain features of the visual or auditory representations from which they were derived. However, there was no reliable evidence of a performance cost for mixed modalities in the matching pair when the second of the two did not immediately follow the first, suggesting that more enduring spatial images in working memory may be amodal. 相似文献
908.
Repeated acquisition and discrimination reversal tasks are often used to examine behavioral relations of, respectively, learning and cognitive flexibility. Surprisingly, despite their frequent use in cognitive neuroscience and behavioral pharmacology, variables that control performance under these two tasks have not been widely studied. The present studies were conducted to directly investigate the controlling variables in nonhuman primates. Squirrel monkeys were trained with a touchscreen variant of the repeated acquisition task in which a novel pair of S+/S? stimuli was presented daily. Subjects learned to discriminate the two stimuli (acquisition) and, subsequently, with the contingencies switched (reversal). Results indicate that rates of both acquisition and reversal learning increased across successive sessions, but that rate of reversal learning remained slower than acquisition learning, i.e., more trials were needed for mastery. Subsequent experiments showed this difference between the rate of learning novel discriminations and reversal was reliable for at least 5 days between acquisition and reversal and notwithstanding the interpolation of additional discriminations. Experimental analysis of the S+/S? elements of the tasks revealed that the difference in the rate of learning could not be attributed to a relatively aversive quality of the S? or to a relatively appetitive quality of the S+, but, rather, to contextual control by the S+/S? stimulus complex. Thus, if either element (S+ or S?) of the stimulus complex was replaced by a novel stimulus, the rate of acquisition approximated that expected with a novel stimulus pair. These results improve our understanding of fundamental features of discrimination acquisition and reversal. 相似文献
909.
Paul S. Bradley Christopher Carling Antonio Gomez Diaz Peter Hood Chris Barnes Jack Ade Mark Boddy Peter Krustrup Magni Mohr 《Human movement science》2013,32(4):808-821
The aim of this study was to compare the match performance and physical capacity of players in the top three competitive standards of English soccer. Match performance data were collected from players in the FA Premier League (n = 190), Championship (n = 155) and League 1 (n = 366) using a multiple-camera system. In addition, a selection of players from the Premier League (n = 56), Championship (n = 61) and League 1 (n = 32) performed the Yo-Yo intermittent endurance test level 2 (Yo-Yo IE2) to determine physical capacity. Players in League 1 and the Championship performed more (p < .01) high-intensity running than those in the Premier League (Effect Size [ES]: 0.4–1.0). Technical indicators such as pass completion, frequency of forward and total passes, balls received and average touches per possession were 4–39% higher (p < .01) in the Premier League compared to lower standards (ES: 0.3–0.6). Players also covered more (p < .05) high-intensity running when moving down (n = 20) from the Premier League to the Championship (ES: 0.4) but not when players moved up (n = 18) standards (ES: 0.2). Similar Yo-Yo IE2 test performances were observed in Premier League, Championship and League 1 players (ES: 0.2–0.3). Large magnitude relationships (p < .05) were observed between Yo-Yo IE2 test performances and the total and high-intensity running distance covered in both Championship (r = .56 and .64) and Premier League matches (r = .61 and .54). The data demonstrate that high-intensity running distance was greater in players at lower compared to higher competitive standards despite a similar physical capacity in a subsample of players in each standard. These findings could be associated with technical characteristics inherent to lower standards that require players to tax their physical capacity to a greater extent but additional research is still required to confirm these findings. 相似文献
910.
John R. Lubker Amanda J. Visek Jack C. Watson II Darius Singpurwalla 《Journal of Applied Sport Psychology》2013,25(4):465-480
Studies assessing the perceived characteristics of competent sport psychology practitioners have resulted in skewed data, making it difficult to discern the relative importance of each characteristic. Therefore, the purpose of this study was to assess the attributes of gender, ethnicity, interpersonal skills, physical appearance, athletic background, professional status, and sport culture in relation to one another using conjoint analysis, a consumer market analysis. College student athletes (N = 464) indicated a preference for a female practitioner whose ethnicity matched that of the athlete and possessed high interpersonal skills and sport knowledge, an athletic background, a physically fit body type, and had earned a professional degree. These findings can be used by sport psychology consultants to modify the information they give to current clients and to develop marketing plans to appeal to potential clients. 相似文献