首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   121篇
  免费   5篇
  国内免费   1篇
  127篇
  2022年   1篇
  2020年   3篇
  2019年   4篇
  2018年   1篇
  2017年   4篇
  2016年   2篇
  2015年   4篇
  2014年   4篇
  2013年   14篇
  2012年   3篇
  2011年   1篇
  2010年   2篇
  2009年   4篇
  2008年   2篇
  2007年   3篇
  2006年   2篇
  2005年   6篇
  2004年   2篇
  2003年   5篇
  2002年   3篇
  2001年   3篇
  2000年   1篇
  1999年   1篇
  1998年   3篇
  1997年   2篇
  1996年   1篇
  1995年   2篇
  1994年   2篇
  1992年   1篇
  1991年   3篇
  1990年   2篇
  1988年   2篇
  1987年   3篇
  1984年   3篇
  1983年   5篇
  1982年   3篇
  1981年   3篇
  1979年   1篇
  1975年   3篇
  1974年   1篇
  1973年   2篇
  1972年   1篇
  1970年   3篇
  1969年   3篇
  1968年   2篇
  1964年   1篇
排序方式: 共有127条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
1.
Jaakko Hintikka 《Synthese》1990,83(1):133-157
This paper was first read at the 1988 Annual Meeting of the International Institute of Philosophy in Stockholm, Sweden. The theme of the meeting was Descartes and the Contemporary Philosophy of Mind, which helped to explain the angle of my approach and the first few sentences of the paper. Morally, this paper thus should appear with the rest of the proceedings of the Stockholm meeting in a later number of this journal. However, the interrelations of my paper and Lucia Vaina's are so close that it makes best sense to publish them together.  相似文献   
2.
3.
4.
5.
As part of a larger research project on couple therapy for depression, this qualitative case study examines the nature of dialogue. Drawing on Bakhtinian concepts, the investigation shows how the conversation shifts from a monologue to dialogue. Among the findings are: first, the process of listening is integral to the transforming experience. That is, the careful listening of the therapist can evoke new voices, just as the experience of one of the partners’ “listening in” to the conversation between the other partner and the therapist can create movement and new trajectories. The latter is a qualitative difference between dialogic therapy with a couple and that with an individual. Second, the therapist not only acts as creative listener, but as the dialogue unfolds, actively contributes to meaning‐making. Third, the study upholds having a team of researchers as a polyphonic forum and the usefulness of Bakhtinian concepts in clinical research on dialogue in multi‐actor sessions.  相似文献   
6.
Parents who came with their child to Child and Adolescent Psychiatry (CAP) for the first time were interviewed 1–2 weeks afterwards in the presence of the child and the therapists. In a grounded theory analysis they revealed uncertainty about their role in the first meeting as well as of future planning. What had been important to them were aspects like communication, sharing perspectives, and the reformulation of problems.  相似文献   
7.
本文介绍的两个“芬兰双生子(Finn Twin)”研究是对10个出生组的青少年双生子及其家庭的长期跟踪研究。本文概述了这两个项目对遗传与环境的关联和互动对吸烟饮酒行为影响的研究。这些研究的结果提示个体差异对吸烟饮酒行为的长期变动的影响受到兄弟姐妹互动、同伴互动以及父母教养方式的调节。环境对吸烟饮酒的影响主要表现在学校、居住社区与家庭环境的差异上。环境与遗传影响的程度和持久度有城乡差异。遗传的影响受居住社区的制约。多阶段模型的分析揭示了遗传与环境的互动对这些行为的演变(从试吸试饮到发展到滥用)的动态的影响,遗传似乎对吸烟饮酒的共同轨迹更有影响。这些结果对今后本项目对这约1万对芬兰成年双生子的分子遗传学研究提供了有益的线索  相似文献   
8.
9.
Omitting experimental data is often considered a violation of scientific integrity. If we consider experimental inquiry as a questioning process, omitting data is seen to be merely an example of tentatively rejecting (‘bracketing’) some of nature’s answers. Such bracketing is not only occasionally permissible; sometimes it is mandated by optimal interrogative strategies. When to omit data is therefore a strategic rather than ethical question. These points are illustrated by reference to Millikan’s oil drop experiment.  相似文献   
10.
It does take a village: nonfamilial environments and children's behavior   总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3  
Abstract - Family characteristics influence children's behavioral development, but so do variations in schools, neighborhoods, and communities. We documented extrafamilial environmental effects by fitting maximum likelihood models to questionnaire data collected from double dyads consisting of twins and their classmate controls. The classmate controls in each double dyad were genetic strangers living in separate households, but they shared school, neighborhood, and community environments with their yoked twin pair and with one another. At ages 11 to 12, the control classmates showed significant similarities in religious practices and smoking and drinking patterns, demonstrating that environmental influences outside the family affect children's behavioral development. Familial self-selection of residential neighborhoods may have contributed to these results, but direct effects of variation across communities, neighborhoods, and schools cannot be dismissed, and such effects warrant further study.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号