首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   2407篇
  免费   37篇
  2444篇
  2019年   24篇
  2018年   22篇
  2017年   21篇
  2016年   37篇
  2014年   28篇
  2013年   246篇
  2012年   59篇
  2011年   60篇
  2010年   43篇
  2009年   68篇
  2008年   58篇
  2007年   69篇
  2006年   55篇
  2005年   52篇
  2004年   67篇
  2003年   65篇
  2002年   74篇
  2001年   30篇
  2000年   36篇
  1999年   30篇
  1998年   36篇
  1997年   39篇
  1996年   29篇
  1995年   40篇
  1994年   37篇
  1993年   24篇
  1992年   33篇
  1991年   28篇
  1990年   31篇
  1989年   29篇
  1988年   27篇
  1987年   25篇
  1986年   33篇
  1985年   31篇
  1984年   41篇
  1983年   41篇
  1982年   38篇
  1981年   55篇
  1980年   47篇
  1979年   51篇
  1978年   39篇
  1977年   37篇
  1976年   47篇
  1975年   54篇
  1974年   50篇
  1973年   42篇
  1972年   24篇
  1971年   30篇
  1969年   35篇
  1968年   34篇
排序方式: 共有2444条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
951.
The impact of abrupt-onset cues on memory search was investigated, where the abrupt-onset cue was a valid (Experiment 1), random (Experiment 2), or irrelevant (Experiment 3) predictor of the location containing the test probe. In Experiment 4, the abrupt-onset cue either preceded or followed the test probe. Sternberg-like functions were obtained in Experiments 1 and 2, with the effects of the abrupt-onset cue localized primarily in the intercept rather than the slope. Experiment 3 demonstrated that a spatially separated and irrelevant abrupt-onset cue increased latency even when all memory probes occurred at the fixation point. In Experiment 4, the robust impact of an abrupt-onset cue vanished, regardless of stimulus onset asynchrony, when it followed the target. We concluded that abrupt-onset cues captured attention regardless of their predictability, manifested as a delaying of search. However, once attention was captured by the target, a subsequent abrupt-onset stimulus had no effect. These results were discussed in terms of diffuse attention and contingent capture models of attention.  相似文献   
952.
This study assessed the factor structure, internal consistency, and concurrent validity of the Smoking Expectancies Scale for Adolescents (SESA) using 717 Australian adolescents (87% nonsmokers, 11% current smokers, and 2% ex-smokers). Exploratory factor analysis of SESA yielded 8 factors. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the 8-factor model, and also a 2nd-order cost-benefit model, fit the data significantly better than 4 alternatives. Validation analyses revealed the 8-factor model explained 26% to 32% of the variance in adolescent cigarette use, smoking intentions, smoking subjective norms, and peer smoking. The 2nd-order model explained 12% to 17% of the variance in these same variables.  相似文献   
953.
954.
Donald Regan 《Synthese》1982,53(2):243-249
  相似文献   
955.
We review the major phenomena of skilled typing and propose a model for the control of the hands and fingers during typing. The model is based upon an Activation-Trigger-Schema system in which a hierarchical structure of schemata directs the selection of the letters to be typed and, then, controls the hand and finger movements by a cooperative, relaxation algorithm. The interactions of the patterns of activation and inhibition among the schemata determine the temporal ordering for launching the keystrokes. To account for the phenomena of doubling errors, the model has only “type” schemata—no “token” schemata—with only a weak binding between the special schema that signals a doubling, and its argument. The model exists as a working computer simulation and produces an output display of the hands and fingers moving over the keyboard. It reproduces some of the major phenomena of typing, including the interkeystroke interval times, the pattern of transposition errors found in skilled typists, and doubling errors. Although the model is clearly inadequate or wrong in some of its features and assumptions, it serves as a useful first approximation for the understanding of skilled typing.  相似文献   
956.
957.
958.
959.
960.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号