全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2407篇 |
免费 | 37篇 |
专业分类
2444篇 |
出版年
2019年 | 24篇 |
2018年 | 22篇 |
2017年 | 21篇 |
2016年 | 37篇 |
2014年 | 28篇 |
2013年 | 246篇 |
2012年 | 59篇 |
2011年 | 60篇 |
2010年 | 43篇 |
2009年 | 68篇 |
2008年 | 58篇 |
2007年 | 69篇 |
2006年 | 55篇 |
2005年 | 52篇 |
2004年 | 67篇 |
2003年 | 65篇 |
2002年 | 74篇 |
2001年 | 30篇 |
2000年 | 36篇 |
1999年 | 30篇 |
1998年 | 36篇 |
1997年 | 39篇 |
1996年 | 29篇 |
1995年 | 40篇 |
1994年 | 37篇 |
1993年 | 24篇 |
1992年 | 33篇 |
1991年 | 28篇 |
1990年 | 31篇 |
1989年 | 29篇 |
1988年 | 27篇 |
1987年 | 25篇 |
1986年 | 33篇 |
1985年 | 31篇 |
1984年 | 41篇 |
1983年 | 41篇 |
1982年 | 38篇 |
1981年 | 55篇 |
1980年 | 47篇 |
1979年 | 51篇 |
1978年 | 39篇 |
1977年 | 37篇 |
1976年 | 47篇 |
1975年 | 54篇 |
1974年 | 50篇 |
1973年 | 42篇 |
1972年 | 24篇 |
1971年 | 30篇 |
1969年 | 35篇 |
1968年 | 34篇 |
排序方式: 共有2444条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
951.
The impact of abrupt-onset cues on memory search was investigated, where the abrupt-onset cue was a valid (Experiment 1), random (Experiment 2), or irrelevant (Experiment 3) predictor of the location containing the test probe. In Experiment 4, the abrupt-onset cue either preceded or followed the test probe. Sternberg-like functions were obtained in Experiments 1 and 2, with the effects of the abrupt-onset cue localized primarily in the intercept rather than the slope. Experiment 3 demonstrated that a spatially separated and irrelevant abrupt-onset cue increased latency even when all memory probes occurred at the fixation point. In Experiment 4, the robust impact of an abrupt-onset cue vanished, regardless of stimulus onset asynchrony, when it followed the target. We concluded that abrupt-onset cues captured attention regardless of their predictability, manifested as a delaying of search. However, once attention was captured by the target, a subsequent abrupt-onset stimulus had no effect. These results were discussed in terms of diffuse attention and contingent capture models of attention. 相似文献
952.
This study assessed the factor structure, internal consistency, and concurrent validity of the Smoking Expectancies Scale for Adolescents (SESA) using 717 Australian adolescents (87% nonsmokers, 11% current smokers, and 2% ex-smokers). Exploratory factor analysis of SESA yielded 8 factors. Confirmatory factor analysis indicated that the 8-factor model, and also a 2nd-order cost-benefit model, fit the data significantly better than 4 alternatives. Validation analyses revealed the 8-factor model explained 26% to 32% of the variance in adolescent cigarette use, smoking intentions, smoking subjective norms, and peer smoking. The 2nd-order model explained 12% to 17% of the variance in these same variables. 相似文献
953.
954.
955.
We review the major phenomena of skilled typing and propose a model for the control of the hands and fingers during typing. The model is based upon an Activation-Trigger-Schema system in which a hierarchical structure of schemata directs the selection of the letters to be typed and, then, controls the hand and finger movements by a cooperative, relaxation algorithm. The interactions of the patterns of activation and inhibition among the schemata determine the temporal ordering for launching the keystrokes. To account for the phenomena of doubling errors, the model has only “type” schemata—no “token” schemata—with only a weak binding between the special schema that signals a doubling, and its argument. The model exists as a working computer simulation and produces an output display of the hands and fingers moving over the keyboard. It reproduces some of the major phenomena of typing, including the interkeystroke interval times, the pattern of transposition errors found in skilled typists, and doubling errors. Although the model is clearly inadequate or wrong in some of its features and assumptions, it serves as a useful first approximation for the understanding of skilled typing. 相似文献
956.
957.
958.
959.
960.