全文获取类型
收费全文 | 257篇 |
免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
258篇 |
出版年
2017年 | 2篇 |
2014年 | 3篇 |
2013年 | 21篇 |
2012年 | 5篇 |
2011年 | 3篇 |
2010年 | 4篇 |
2009年 | 8篇 |
2008年 | 8篇 |
2007年 | 4篇 |
2006年 | 4篇 |
2005年 | 4篇 |
2002年 | 4篇 |
2001年 | 3篇 |
2000年 | 2篇 |
1999年 | 4篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 5篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 2篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 7篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 5篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 2篇 |
1977年 | 4篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 3篇 |
1966年 | 3篇 |
1962年 | 3篇 |
1959年 | 3篇 |
1958年 | 12篇 |
1957年 | 7篇 |
1956年 | 12篇 |
1955年 | 5篇 |
1954年 | 12篇 |
1953年 | 4篇 |
1952年 | 3篇 |
1951年 | 5篇 |
1950年 | 4篇 |
1949年 | 4篇 |
1948年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有258条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
111.
MYLAN ENGEL JR. 《The Southern journal of philosophy》2012,50(1):50-74
This paper defends a coherentist approach to moral epistemology. In “The Immorality of Eating Meat” (2000), I offer a coherentist consistency argument to show that our own beliefs rationally commit us to the immorality of eating meat. Elsewhere, I use our own beliefs as premises to argue that we have positive duties to assist the poor (2004) and to argue that biomedical animal experimentation is wrong (2012). The present paper explores whether this consistency‐based coherentist approach of grounding particular moral judgments on beliefs we already hold, with no appeal to moral theory, is a legitimate way of doing practical ethics. I argue (i) that grounding particular moral judgments on our core moral convictions and other core nonmoral beliefs is a legitimate way to justify moral judgments, (ii) that these moral judgments possess as much epistemic justification and have as much claim to objectivity as moral judgments grounded on particular ethical theories, and (iii) that this internalistic coherentist method of grounding moral judgments is more likely to result in behavioral guidance than traditional theory‐based approaches to practical ethics. By way of illustrating the approach, I briefly recapitulate my consistency‐based argument for ethical vegetarianism. I then defend the coherentist approach implicit in the argument against a number of potentially fatal metatheoretical attacks. 相似文献
112.
Dr JR Pedder MA MRCP FRCPsych 《Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy》2013,27(3):201-217
Freud's 1919 paper is taken as a starting-point to review how far we have advanced towards fulfilment of his prophetic remarks about the need to develop psychoanalytic psychotherapy for the masses. A glance is taken back to some of the circumstances and characters involved in Budapest where the paper was given at the 1918 congress, where there was much talk about the need to do something about the war neuroses. Similar pressures led to the founding of the Tavistock Clinic and Cassel Hospital in the UK, long before the start of the NHS. The pressures of both world wars advanced the applications of psychoanalysis; the next wave of pressures may come from NHS changes and entry into Europe. The history of psychoanalytic psychotherapy in the UK is also considered via looking briefly at the history and development of psychoanalytic publications, of the Tavistock Clinic, of psychoanalytic training, of child psychotherapy and the emergence of adult (non-medical) psychotherapy. There have always been tensions between exponents of the pure gold of analysis and the needs of its wider applications. More flexible arrangements and boundaries are necessary if we are to spread psychoanalysis beyond London and to fulfil Freud's own hopes about the large-scale application of psychoanalytic therapy. 相似文献
113.
WATCHING THE DETECTIVES: SEASONAL STUDENT EMPLOYEE REACTIONS TO ELECTRONIC MONITORING WITH AND WITHOUT ADVANCE NOTIFICATION 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
AUDRA D. HOVORKA-MEAD WILUAM H. ROSS JR TRACY WHIPPLE MICHELLA B. RENCHIN 《Personnel Psychology》2002,55(2):329-362
The present paper tested procedural justice hypotheses about seasonal high school and college student employees' reactions to electronic monitoring with video cameras. Study 1, a field study, explored (a) whether employees receiving advance notification of monitoring offered more favorable justice judgments than employees who did not, and (b) whether employees who saw monitoring procedures and/or consequences as fair returned to the organization the following summer. Results supported the hypotheses: employees viewed monitoring procedures as fairer if they received advance notice. Fairness judgments predicted reemployment Study 2, a scenario-based laboratory experiment, also found that advance notice elicited greater justice beliefs. In addition, Study 2 examined how variations in justification for the monitoring affected justice beliefs. Either strong or weak justifications produced greater procedural justice beliefs than no justification. 相似文献
114.
115.
116.
117.
118.
119.
Affective style (AS) and communication deviance (CD) have been suggested as markers of dysfunctional family environments that may be associated with psychiatric illness. Studies have focused mainly on parental responses during family interactions when an offspring is the identified patient. The present study is unique in examining AS and CD in mothers with unipolar depression, bipolar disorder, or chronic physical illness, and in normal controls. The sample consisted of 64 mothers with children ages 8 to 16. Unipolar mothers were more likely to show negative AS than were any other maternal group. There were no group differences for CD. Chronic stress, few positive life events, and single parenting were associated with AS. CD was associated solely with lower socioeconomic status. Results suggest that dysfunctional interactions are determined not only by maternal psychopathology, but also by an array of contextual factors that are related to the quality of the family environment. 相似文献
120.
This study addresses the question of whether developmental assessment centers are effective interventions. Career outcomes of assessment center participants ( n = 151) were evaluated with respect to center ratings, developmental activity, and acceptance of feedback. Although comparison between a subgroup of assessees ( n = 113) and a naturally occurring control group of non-assessees ( n = 167) showed no differences in terms of career advancement, findings within the assessee sample did show that developmental activity related to advancement. Findings also showed that assessors' ratings of career motivation, rather than ratings of knowledge or skill (KS) dimensions, predicted both developmental activity and career advancement. 相似文献