全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4552篇 |
免费 | 269篇 |
专业分类
4821篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2023年 | 4篇 |
2022年 | 3篇 |
2021年 | 81篇 |
2020年 | 104篇 |
2019年 | 404篇 |
2018年 | 267篇 |
2017年 | 391篇 |
2016年 | 375篇 |
2015年 | 355篇 |
2014年 | 310篇 |
2013年 | 510篇 |
2012年 | 279篇 |
2011年 | 207篇 |
2010年 | 241篇 |
2009年 | 215篇 |
2008年 | 196篇 |
2007年 | 119篇 |
2006年 | 108篇 |
2005年 | 113篇 |
2004年 | 88篇 |
2003年 | 92篇 |
2002年 | 97篇 |
2001年 | 83篇 |
2000年 | 67篇 |
1999年 | 33篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 5篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1984年 | 2篇 |
1983年 | 1篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1975年 | 1篇 |
1974年 | 2篇 |
1973年 | 2篇 |
1972年 | 4篇 |
1962年 | 1篇 |
1944年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有4821条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
191.
Nava Levit‐Binnun Ohad Szepsenwol Keren Stern‐Ellran Batya Engel‐Yeger 《Australian journal of psychology》2014,66(4):233-240
Individual variations in the way people respond to sensory stimuli can sometimes lead to maladaptive representations of the world. Indeed, sensory responsiveness profiles were found to be associated with mood symptoms such as depression and anxiety. The goal of the current study was to investigate whether attachment orientations can account for the relationship between sensory responsiveness profiles and anxiety symptoms. Participants (N = 194) completed a battery of questionnaires assessing sensory responsiveness profiles, attachment orientations, and anxiety symptoms. As expected, various associations between sensory responsiveness profiles and anxiety symptoms were accounted for by attachment anxiety and avoidance. We suggest a possible causal path, in which early‐developing sensory responsiveness profiles lead to attachment insecurities, which in turn may lead to mood symptoms such as anxiety. 相似文献
192.
Estimation of the predictive power of the model in mixed‐effects meta‐regression: A simulation study
José Antonio López‐López Fulgencio Marín‐Martínez Julio Sánchez‐Meca Wim Van den Noortgate Wolfgang Viechtbauer 《The British journal of mathematical and statistical psychology》2014,67(1):30-48
Several methods are available to estimate the total and residual amount of heterogeneity in meta‐analysis, leading to different alternatives when estimating the predictive power in mixed‐effects meta‐regression models using the formula proposed by Raudenbush (1994, 2009). In this paper, a simulation study was conducted to compare the performance of seven estimators of these parameters under various realistic scenarios in psychology and related fields. Our results suggest that the number of studies (k) exerts the most important influence on the accuracy of the results, and that precise estimates of the heterogeneity variances and the model predictive power can only be expected with at least 20 and 40 studies, respectively. Increases in the average within‐study sample size () also improved the results for all estimators. Some differences among the accuracy of the estimators were observed, especially under adverse (small k and ) conditions, while the results for the different methods tended to convergence for more optimal scenarios. 相似文献
193.
194.
195.
196.
Nicolas Benguigui Michael P. Broderick Robin Baurès Michel‐Ange Amorim 《The British journal of developmental psychology》2008,26(3):389-407
In coincidence‐timing studies, children have been shown to respond too early to slower stimuli and too late to faster stimuli. To examine this velocity effect, children aged 6, 7.5, 9, 10.5, and adults were tested with two different velocities in a prediction‐motion task which consisted of judging, after the occlusion of the final part of its path, the moment of arrival of a moving stimulus towards a specified position. A similar velocity effect, resulting in later responses for the faster velocities than for the slower, was found primarily in the three younger groups of children (for the longer occlusion conditions: 600–1,320 milliseconds). However, this effect was not seen in all children in these groups. Individual analyses showed that this velocity effect, when present, is linked to the use of distance rather than time information, or to the confusion between these in extrapolating the occluded trajectories. The tendency to use one type of information or the other is a good predictor of accuracy and variability in this task and a good indicator of the development stage of the participants. Across development, children tend to initially use distance information with poor accuracy but relative consistency in responses. In a second stage, they use time and distance information alternatively across trials trying to find a better source of information with still poor accuracy and now great variability. In a final stage, they use time information to reach consistency and accuracy in their responses. This chronology follows the stages proposed by Savelesbergh and Van der Kamp (2000) explaining development with an initial stage of ‘freezing’ non‐optimal relationships between information and movement, then a ‘freeing’ stage during which new solutions are searched for, and finally an ‘exploiting’ stage with an optimal relationship between information and movement. 相似文献
197.
Nele De Cuyper Claudia Bernhard‐Oettel Erik Berntson Hans De Witte Barbara Alarco 《Psychologie appliquee》2008,57(3):488-509
The current study's aims are twofold: first, we investigate the relationship between employability and both work‐related (engagement) and general (life satisfaction) well‐being. Second, we study how employability may be relevant in times of high job insecurity. Specifically, we hypothesise (1) a positive relationship between employability and employees’ well‐being, (2) a negative relationship between employability and job insecurity, and (3) a negative relationship between job insecurity and employees’ well‐being, so that (4) job insecurity mediates the relationship between employability and employees’ well‐being. Results based on a sample of 559 respondents from divisions of seven Belgian organisations support our hypotheses. We conclude that employability may be a means to secure one's labour market position, rather than a means to cope with job insecurity. Les objectifs de cette étude sont doubles: nous étudions d’abord la relation entre l’employabilité et à la fois le bien‐être relatif au travail (implication) et le bien‐être global (la satisfaction provenant de l’existence). Ensuite, nous nous interrogeons sur la pertinence de l’employabilité en période de grande insécurité professionnelle. Plus précisément, nous posons les hypothèses 1) d’une relation positive entre l’employabilité et le bien‐être des employés, 2) d’une relation négative entre l’employabilité et l’insécurité professionnelle et 3) d’une relation négative entre l’insécurité professionnelle et le bien‐être des salariés, si bien que 4) l’insécurité professionnelle régule la relation entre l’employabilité et le bien‐être des salariés. Les résultats issus d’un échantillon de 559 répondants provenant de départements de sept organisations belges confortent nos hypothèses. On conclut que l’employabilité peut être une façon de sécuriser sa situation sur le marché du travail plutôt qu’un moyen pour affronter l’insécurité professionnelle. 相似文献
198.
199.
200.