全文获取类型
收费全文 | 102755篇 |
免费 | 4235篇 |
国内免费 | 37篇 |
专业分类
107027篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 1107篇 |
2019年 | 1366篇 |
2018年 | 1892篇 |
2017年 | 1955篇 |
2016年 | 2056篇 |
2015年 | 1479篇 |
2014年 | 1755篇 |
2013年 | 8341篇 |
2012年 | 3287篇 |
2011年 | 3410篇 |
2010年 | 2101篇 |
2009年 | 2118篇 |
2008年 | 2973篇 |
2007年 | 2980篇 |
2006年 | 2731篇 |
2005年 | 2372篇 |
2004年 | 2282篇 |
2003年 | 2173篇 |
2002年 | 2212篇 |
2001年 | 3289篇 |
2000年 | 3150篇 |
1999年 | 2381篇 |
1998年 | 1175篇 |
1997年 | 1043篇 |
1996年 | 1080篇 |
1995年 | 986篇 |
1993年 | 967篇 |
1992年 | 1999篇 |
1991年 | 1868篇 |
1990年 | 1882篇 |
1989年 | 1731篇 |
1988年 | 1689篇 |
1987年 | 1605篇 |
1986年 | 1648篇 |
1985年 | 1704篇 |
1984年 | 1443篇 |
1983年 | 1286篇 |
1982年 | 942篇 |
1979年 | 1515篇 |
1978年 | 1109篇 |
1975年 | 1220篇 |
1974年 | 1355篇 |
1973年 | 1465篇 |
1972年 | 1217篇 |
1971年 | 1143篇 |
1970年 | 1018篇 |
1969年 | 1036篇 |
1968年 | 1303篇 |
1967年 | 1173篇 |
1966年 | 1059篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
42.
Sara J. Shettleworth 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section B: Comparative and Physiological Psychology》1981,33(4):241-255
Are different responses differentially associable with their consequences? An overshadowing design was used to try to answer this question for three responses previously studied in golden hamsters. In Experiment I, scrabbling was rapidly suppressed by electric shock punishment, and it overshadowed a tone which occurred between scrabbling and shock. In Experiment II, no evidence of response-shock association was obtained when open rearing was the punished response, and open rearing did not overshadow the tone. Punishment had some effect on face washing, but there was no statistically significant overshadowing with this response. These results are consistent with the idea that differences in punishment suppression among these three responses have an associative basis. They also demonstrate the usefulness of a novel paradigm for studying “preparedness” of response-reinforcer associations. 相似文献
43.
44.
45.
46.
Vladimir M. Sloutsky Yevgeniya Goldvarg 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》2004,57(4):636-665
Logical connectives, such as “AND”, “OR”, “IF . . . THEN”, and “IF AND ONLY IF” are ubiquitous in both language and cognition; however, reasoning with logical connectives is error-prone. We argue that some of these errors may stem from people's tendency to minimize the number of possibilities compatible with logical connectives and to construct a “minimalist” one-possibility representation. As a result, connectives denoting a single possibility (e.g., conjunctions) are likely to be represented correctly, whereas connectives denoting multiple possibilities (e.g., disjunctions or conditionals) are likely to be erroneously represented as conjunctions. These predictions were tested and confirmed in three experiments using different paradigms. In Experiment 1, participants were presented with a multiple-choice task and asked to select all and only those possibilities that would indicate that compound verbal propositions were true versus false. In Experiment 2, a somewhat similar task was used, except that participants were asked later to perform a cued recall of verbal propositions. Finally, Experiment 3 used an old/new recognition paradigm to examine participants' ability to accurately recognize different logical connectives. The results of the three experiments are discussed in relation to theories of representation of possibilities and theories of reasoning. 相似文献
47.
48.
The ability to make egocentric distance estimates of a single point source of light, seen in darkness and without the cues of changing size and luminance, was investigated in sixteen observers. The attenuation required to maintain constant luminance, when the target was viewed from different distances, was shown to follow the inverse square law providing the angle subtended by the light was less than 20 s arc. Distance changes were also simulated by means of a split mirror which produced vergence cues, or by test lenses to provide accommodation cues. Over the range 0.5 to 9.2 m distance estimates were surprisingly accurate, although there was some overestimation of near and underestimation of far distances. Most observers made good judgements when only convergence cues were varied, whereas no observers made consistently good judgements when only accommodation cues were varied. The difficulties are discussed in terms of the accommodation-convergence link. When distance was simulated by changing convergence and accommodation cues, estimates were not as good as when real distance was changed. Since good estimates were made with brief target exposures, these judgements were not based on subsequent convergence or accommodation changes. It is suggested that the metric or reference against which the apparently absolute judgements were made was the efferent demand signal associated with a 'resting' position of convergence in darkness. 相似文献
49.
50.