首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   102755篇
  免费   4235篇
  国内免费   37篇
  107027篇
  2020年   1107篇
  2019年   1366篇
  2018年   1892篇
  2017年   1955篇
  2016年   2056篇
  2015年   1479篇
  2014年   1755篇
  2013年   8341篇
  2012年   3287篇
  2011年   3410篇
  2010年   2101篇
  2009年   2118篇
  2008年   2973篇
  2007年   2980篇
  2006年   2731篇
  2005年   2372篇
  2004年   2282篇
  2003年   2173篇
  2002年   2212篇
  2001年   3289篇
  2000年   3150篇
  1999年   2381篇
  1998年   1175篇
  1997年   1043篇
  1996年   1080篇
  1995年   986篇
  1993年   967篇
  1992年   1999篇
  1991年   1868篇
  1990年   1882篇
  1989年   1731篇
  1988年   1689篇
  1987年   1605篇
  1986年   1648篇
  1985年   1704篇
  1984年   1443篇
  1983年   1286篇
  1982年   942篇
  1979年   1515篇
  1978年   1109篇
  1975年   1220篇
  1974年   1355篇
  1973年   1465篇
  1972年   1217篇
  1971年   1143篇
  1970年   1018篇
  1969年   1036篇
  1968年   1303篇
  1967年   1173篇
  1966年   1059篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
41.
42.
Are different responses differentially associable with their consequences? An overshadowing design was used to try to answer this question for three responses previously studied in golden hamsters. In Experiment I, scrabbling was rapidly suppressed by electric shock punishment, and it overshadowed a tone which occurred between scrabbling and shock. In Experiment II, no evidence of response-shock association was obtained when open rearing was the punished response, and open rearing did not overshadow the tone. Punishment had some effect on face washing, but there was no statistically significant overshadowing with this response. These results are consistent with the idea that differences in punishment suppression among these three responses have an associative basis. They also demonstrate the usefulness of a novel paradigm for studying “preparedness” of response-reinforcer associations.  相似文献   
43.
44.
45.
46.
Logical connectives, such as “AND”, “OR”, “IF . . . THEN”, and “IF AND ONLY IF” are ubiquitous in both language and cognition; however, reasoning with logical connectives is error-prone. We argue that some of these errors may stem from people's tendency to minimize the number of possibilities compatible with logical connectives and to construct a “minimalist” one-possibility representation. As a result, connectives denoting a single possibility (e.g., conjunctions) are likely to be represented correctly, whereas connectives denoting multiple possibilities (e.g., disjunctions or conditionals) are likely to be erroneously represented as conjunctions. These predictions were tested and confirmed in three experiments using different paradigms. In Experiment 1, participants were presented with a multiple-choice task and asked to select all and only those possibilities that would indicate that compound verbal propositions were true versus false. In Experiment 2, a somewhat similar task was used, except that participants were asked later to perform a cued recall of verbal propositions. Finally, Experiment 3 used an old/new recognition paradigm to examine participants' ability to accurately recognize different logical connectives. The results of the three experiments are discussed in relation to theories of representation of possibilities and theories of reasoning.  相似文献   
47.
48.
The ability to make egocentric distance estimates of a single point source of light, seen in darkness and without the cues of changing size and luminance, was investigated in sixteen observers. The attenuation required to maintain constant luminance, when the target was viewed from different distances, was shown to follow the inverse square law providing the angle subtended by the light was less than 20 s arc. Distance changes were also simulated by means of a split mirror which produced vergence cues, or by test lenses to provide accommodation cues. Over the range 0.5 to 9.2 m distance estimates were surprisingly accurate, although there was some overestimation of near and underestimation of far distances. Most observers made good judgements when only convergence cues were varied, whereas no observers made consistently good judgements when only accommodation cues were varied. The difficulties are discussed in terms of the accommodation-convergence link. When distance was simulated by changing convergence and accommodation cues, estimates were not as good as when real distance was changed. Since good estimates were made with brief target exposures, these judgements were not based on subsequent convergence or accommodation changes. It is suggested that the metric or reference against which the apparently absolute judgements were made was the efferent demand signal associated with a 'resting' position of convergence in darkness.  相似文献   
49.
50.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号