全文获取类型
收费全文 | 881篇 |
免费 | 31篇 |
国内免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
913篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2023年 | 6篇 |
2022年 | 8篇 |
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 22篇 |
2019年 | 27篇 |
2018年 | 16篇 |
2017年 | 45篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 28篇 |
2014年 | 26篇 |
2013年 | 112篇 |
2012年 | 37篇 |
2011年 | 22篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 26篇 |
2008年 | 18篇 |
2007年 | 32篇 |
2006年 | 31篇 |
2005年 | 27篇 |
2004年 | 27篇 |
2003年 | 19篇 |
2002年 | 21篇 |
2001年 | 24篇 |
2000年 | 20篇 |
1999年 | 13篇 |
1998年 | 15篇 |
1997年 | 11篇 |
1996年 | 15篇 |
1995年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 8篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 7篇 |
1986年 | 8篇 |
1984年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 8篇 |
1980年 | 6篇 |
1979年 | 7篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1975年 | 8篇 |
1974年 | 11篇 |
1973年 | 5篇 |
1972年 | 6篇 |
1971年 | 5篇 |
1970年 | 9篇 |
1969年 | 5篇 |
1968年 | 8篇 |
1967年 | 5篇 |
1966年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有913条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
161.
Joan Elizabeth Neville King Thomas H. Ollendick Eleonora Gullone Bruce Tonge Shaun Watson 《Counselling psychology quarterly》2006,19(2):151-163
Social Anxiety Disorder (SAD) is a heterogeneous and distressing problem for many children and youth. This review focuses on the etiology and maintenance of SAD, and examines research findings in several key areas of investigation: genetic or hereditary factors (twin and family studies), temperament characteristics (behavioural inhibition), and parent–child interactions (attachment, parenting styles). It is concluded that genetic influences, behavioural inhibition, and parent–child interactions play significant and interactive roles in the development and maintenance of SAD. Other influences such as peer relationships, social skills deficits, and traumatic experiences are also acknowledged. Ultimately, an understanding of such pathways should facilitate effective early screening and intervention of children at risk for severe social anxiety. 相似文献
162.
163.
Bridie Scott-Parker Barry Watson Mark J. King 《Transportation Research Part F: Traffic Psychology and Behaviour》2009,12(6):470-482
Young people aged 17–24 years are at high risk of being killed in road crashes around the world. Road safety interventions consider some influences upon young driver behaviour; for example, imposing passenger restrictions on young novice drivers indirectly minimises the potential negative social influences of peers as passengers. To change young driver risky behaviour, the multitude of psychosocial influences upon its initiation and maintenance must be identified. A study questionnaire was developed to investigate the relationships between risky driving and Akers’ social learning theory, social identity theory, and thrill seeking variables. The questionnaire was completed by 165 participants (105 women, 60 men) residing in south-east Queensland, Australia. The sociodemographic variables of age, gender, and exposure explained 19% of the variance in self-reported risky driving behaviour, whilst Akers’ social learning variables explained an additional 42%. Thrill seeking and social identity variables did not explain any significant additional variance. Significant predictors of risky driving included imitation of the driving behaviours of, and anticipated rewards and punishments administered by, parents and peers. Road safety policy that directly considers and incorporates these factors in their design, implementation, and enforcement of young driver road safety interventions should prove more efficacious than current approaches. 相似文献
164.
165.
Mark J. Landau Daniel Sullivan Laura A. King 《Social and Personality Psychology Compass》2010,4(10):906-917
Drawing on terror management theory (TMT), we discuss the psychological motivations that shape personality at two levels: the characteristically human personality common to us all and the individual differences that distinguish some people from others. TMT posits that the motivation to protect the self against deep-rooted fears about mortality drives people to maintain meaningful, reliable conceptions of reality and positive evaluations of themselves, two broad tendencies that form the foundation of every person’s personality. We review studies showing that mortality reminders increase efforts to bolster cultural sources of meaning and self-esteem in similar ways across individuals and cultures. TMT also posits that individual differences in personality partly reflect the different sources of meaning and self-esteem that people invest in to assuage mortality fears. We review evidence that individual differences predict the degree and direction of people’s defensive responses to mortality reminders. Directions for future research are discussed. 相似文献
166.
167.
In this study the Revised Fear Survey Schedule for Children was administered to 138 hearing-impaired and 134 normally hearing youths. Although both groups expressed fear of potentially dangerous stimuli, significant differences in responding were evident towards other kinds of stimuli. Whereas hearing children were more fearful of failure and criticism, the hearing-impaired children were more afraid of the unknown, injury and small animals. Girls reported significantly greater levels of fear than boys thus confirming a well established research finding. Further, significant interactions were found between hearing status and gender. In contrast to previous findings, age was not related to self-reports of fear. 相似文献
168.
King Seth A. Dzenga Chaidamoyo Burch Taneal Kennedy Krystal 《Journal of Behavioral Education》2021,30(2):202-225
Journal of Behavioral Education - Virtual reality (VR) places individuals within a simulated experience using an array of visual, auditory, and tactile interfaces. Research suggests VR, which... 相似文献
169.
170.
Initial identification discriminations between two sizes and between two slants produced better overall performances on subsequent size and slant same-different discriminations, respectively. This size- and slant-specific transfer was due to an improvement on only the different pairs. Time-duration identification discriminations with the identical stimuli and response assignments improved neither overall same-different performances nor performances on different pairs. A good performance on different pairs relative to same pairs should indicate a low perceived similarity. The literature indicates that A-X and B-Y pairings produce a positive transfer on an A-versus-B discrimination when X and Y are relatively low in similarity, and also indicates that a low perceived (not physical) similarity improves discrimination learning. An increase in salience should have also improved performance on the same pairs. The conclusion: the initial discriminations decreased the perceived similarity of parts (size or slant). This decrease resembles perceptual contrast. A discrimination between two parts also seems to increase the extent to which each part is apprehended as a separate group. Therefore, the conclusion accords with the position that two groups are associated with contrast, including for visibility. 相似文献