首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   78425篇
  免费   3103篇
  国内免费   26篇
  81554篇
  2019年   909篇
  2018年   1258篇
  2017年   1290篇
  2016年   1372篇
  2015年   999篇
  2014年   1209篇
  2013年   5782篇
  2012年   2219篇
  2011年   2407篇
  2010年   1511篇
  2009年   1483篇
  2008年   2145篇
  2007年   2135篇
  2006年   1963篇
  2005年   1720篇
  2004年   1623篇
  2003年   1558篇
  2002年   1671篇
  2001年   2400篇
  2000年   2358篇
  1999年   1816篇
  1998年   896篇
  1997年   840篇
  1996年   839篇
  1995年   754篇
  1992年   1574篇
  1991年   1452篇
  1990年   1442篇
  1989年   1331篇
  1988年   1323篇
  1987年   1246篇
  1986年   1366篇
  1985年   1369篇
  1984年   1165篇
  1983年   1056篇
  1982年   758篇
  1981年   769篇
  1979年   1236篇
  1978年   916篇
  1976年   757篇
  1975年   1027篇
  1974年   1153篇
  1973年   1263篇
  1972年   1029篇
  1971年   1002篇
  1970年   901篇
  1969年   946篇
  1968年   1184篇
  1967年   1066篇
  1966年   974篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
991.
Book Reviews     
The Psychological Record -  相似文献   
992.
An examination of the social perception literature yields little evidence for the false-uniqueness phenomenon (Valins & Nisbett, 1972), the perception that one's attributes are more unique than is the case. In contrast, the tendency for individuals to project their own characteristics onto other people and assume that more people are like themselves is a robust phenomenon. One reason researchers may not have found false uniqueness is that they have not looked at the accuracy of consensus estimates. A close look at the results of Tabachnik, Crocker, and Alloy (1983) and Sanders and Mullen (1983), who did assess accuracy, suggests that people possessing undesirable attributes over-estimate consensus, whereas people holding desirable attributes underestimate consensus. The latter pattern is a form of false uniqueness. In this study we looked at the accuracy of social consensus estimates in the context of psychological fears. A sample of subjects filled out an abbreviated version of a fear survey and made estimates of consensus. The results showed that both high- and low-fear respondents overestimated the incidence of high fear among their peers, but high-fear subjects were more inaccurate in their estimates. A false-uniqueness effect was found on the part of low-fear subjects, as they tended to underestimate the incidence of low fear among their peers. These findings are consistent with a motivational interpretation that emphasizes the individual's need to justify or normalize stigmatized behavior and to bolster perceived self-competence.  相似文献   
993.
Intellectual functioning and aggression   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
In a 22-year study, data were collected on aggressiveness and intellectual functioning in more than 600 subjects, their parents, and their children. Both aggression and intellectual functioning are reasonably stable in a subject's lifetime and perpetuate themselves across generations and within marriage pairs. Aggression in childhood was shown to interfere with the development of intellectual functioning and to be predictive of poorer intellectual achievement as an adult. Early IQ was related to early subject aggression but did not predict changes in aggression after age 8. On the other hand, differences between early IQ and intellectual achievement in middle adulthood were predictable from early aggressive behavior. A dual-process model was offered to explain the relation between intellectual functioning and aggressive behavior. We hypothesized that low intelligence makes the learning of aggressive responses more likely at an early age, and this aggressive behavior makes continued intellectual development more difficult.  相似文献   
994.
In this study we combined daily diary data with interview data to investigate individual differences in the impact of stressful daily events on mood. Using a sample of 96 women in an urban community, we examined perceived neighborhood quality and major life events as possibly potentiating the effects of stressful daily events, and we viewed social supports as potentially buffering this daily process. Results confirmed that the presence of chronic ecologic stress (neighborhood perceptions) exacerbated the immediate effects of stressful daily events on mood and also increased the likelihood of enduring effects of daily stressors on next day's mood. Contrary to expectations, previous exposure to major life events decreased the impact of stressful daily events. The availability of social supports, although not buffering the impact of stressful daily events on mood, did mitigate the enduring effects of these events on next day's mood. This study also presents a method for analyzing daily time-series data, while correcting for potential problems of autocorrelated error terms. As such, this study represents a significant advance over previous analytic approaches to time-series data in the study of the stress process.  相似文献   
995.
996.
997.
998.
A model for stimulus generalization in Pavlovian conditioning   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
  相似文献   
999.
1000.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号