全文获取类型
收费全文 | 61410篇 |
免费 | 1295篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
62708篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 575篇 |
2019年 | 770篇 |
2018年 | 996篇 |
2017年 | 1042篇 |
2016年 | 1110篇 |
2015年 | 812篇 |
2014年 | 951篇 |
2013年 | 4400篇 |
2012年 | 1773篇 |
2011年 | 1927篇 |
2010年 | 1212篇 |
2009年 | 1182篇 |
2008年 | 1696篇 |
2007年 | 1729篇 |
2006年 | 1569篇 |
2005年 | 1381篇 |
2004年 | 1282篇 |
2003年 | 1204篇 |
2002年 | 1314篇 |
2001年 | 1987篇 |
2000年 | 1954篇 |
1999年 | 1480篇 |
1998年 | 700篇 |
1997年 | 618篇 |
1996年 | 619篇 |
1993年 | 560篇 |
1992年 | 1248篇 |
1991年 | 1155篇 |
1990年 | 1133篇 |
1989年 | 1034篇 |
1988年 | 1017篇 |
1987年 | 969篇 |
1986年 | 1049篇 |
1985年 | 1058篇 |
1984年 | 888篇 |
1983年 | 814篇 |
1982年 | 564篇 |
1981年 | 570篇 |
1979年 | 952篇 |
1978年 | 683篇 |
1975年 | 775篇 |
1974年 | 819篇 |
1973年 | 919篇 |
1972年 | 775篇 |
1971年 | 728篇 |
1970年 | 648篇 |
1969年 | 672篇 |
1968年 | 859篇 |
1967年 | 775篇 |
1966年 | 656篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
211.
Developmental levels of processing in metaphor interpretation 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
We outline a theory of metaphor interpretation. The theory posits varying levels of semantic processing and formalizes them in terms of kinds of semantic-mapping operators that transform properties of the metaphoric vehicle (i.e., predicate) into properties of the metaphoric topic (i.e., subject). We used cognitive-developmental theory to estimate the mental-processing complexity of the various mapping operators, and thereby to predict the timing of their emergence in childhood, and to construct a measure of the metaphoric-processing levels. Metaphor interpretations collected from children (aged 6-12 years) and adults were coded and scored according to the processing levels. In two separate developmental studies, processing score increased with age in a predictable way. Growth in a mental-attentional resource accounted for much of the developmental variance in metaphor interpretation. Possible moderating effects of knowledge and context are discussed. 相似文献
212.
213.
Two groups of American undergraduates (moderate and heavy social drinkers) completed a matrix task and received either positive or negative feedback on their performance. Following this they were given a maze task, which was designed so that cheating could be detected. Heavy drinkers cheated more than moderate drinkers under success conditions (positive feedback). Heavy drinkers who received positive feedback also cheated more than heavy drinkers who received negative feedback. The results are interpreted in terms of self-handicapping theory. 相似文献
214.
An Unwillingness to Act: Behavioral Appropriateness, Situational Constraint, and Self-Efficacy in Shyness 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Gretchen J. Hill 《Journal of personality》1989,57(4):871-890
ABSTRACT Three reasons for the failure to demonstrate social skills were examined in an attempt to integrate social skills deficit and dysfunctional cognition approaches to self-labeled shyness To assess social skills knowledge, the willingness to demonstrate such knowledge, and self-efficacy expectations for enacting social behaviors, 40 shy and 40 nonshy adults were asked to judge the appropriateness of situated behaviors and indicate whether they characteristically enacted and felt capable of producing them Results suggested that shys may be unaware of the appropriateness of some behaviors in some situations, however, shys and nonshys more often agree on behavioral appropriateness standards Compared to nonshys, shys are less likely to enact social behaviors and tend to have low self-efficacy It was suggested that shys' self-awareness of characteristic responses and low self-efficacy expectations may contribute to failures to demonstrate social skills knowledge 相似文献
215.
Objective indices (heart rate, perspiration quantity, finger plethysmograph curves, and facial expression) and subjective indices (sensory and emotional subject reports) were recorded for 25 young Chinese adults (16 men, 9 women) during and after the simultaneous application of a strong pain stimulus and either foot classical (alloneural points Tsusanli and Yanglingchuan) or hand classical (alloneural points Hegu and Neiguan) acupuncture. Both forms of acupuncture were equally effective, showing that stimulation of the same nerve is not essential for pain relief. Acupuncture assuaged the emotional, but not the sensory, response to the painful stimulation. 相似文献
216.
JAMES J. KIRK 《Journal of Employment Counseling》1989,26(4):161-168
Type C careerists perceive their second occupations as being more intrinsically, extrinsically, and generally satisfying than they did their initial occupations. 相似文献
217.
Marilyn Likins Charles L. Salzberg Joseph J. Stowitschek Ben Lignugaris/Kraft Rita Curl 《Journal of applied behavior analysis》1989,22(4):381-393
In two studies, co-workers of persons with disabilities were taught to use coincidental training procedures while completing their own jobs. In Study 1, the effects of coincidental training on the salad-making skills of 3 trainees with mild and moderate mental retardation were evaluated. Coincidental training by co-workers resulted in improved accuracy of the salad-making skills of the trainees. In Study 2, trainees were also coincidentally taught to make quality-control checks of their salads. An alternating treatments and multiple baseline design indicated that the trainees more readily acquired the skills when taught to check the correctness of their work. 相似文献
218.
The synchronous paradigm is a model of families whose members remain uninvolved and disconnected from each other yet somehow maintain relatively unvarying or even rigid patterns of behavior. In the synchronous paradigm, calm agreement and harmonious unity of action are valued above all. The concept of synchronous operation was first formulated by Constantine to solve certain theoretical problems growing out of the work of Kantor and Lehr. The theory has been elaborated upon through linkages established with other clinical and theoretical models of family dysfunction. Clinical experience in treating synchronous families has now contributed more detailed and practical understanding. Clinically, disabled synchronous families may present as bland, boring, or even death-like. They may have considerable difficulty accommodating to necessary developmental changes or to the demands of life crises, employing a variety of strategies to maintain the appearance of synchrony at the expense of adaptation. Their ability to ignore change and to absorb interventions can tax the abilities of even the most creative and energetic therapist. On the other hand, the quiet efficiency of the family and the social sensitivity of its members can be strong assets. Awareness of their strengths and special vulnerabilities can help the family therapist work more effectively with synchronous families. 相似文献
219.
To compare the relative effectiveness of three compliance techniques during a museum fund-raising drive, 89 individuals were asked to contribute $1 to the museum after having been exposed to an initial request according to one of the following conditions: (1) in the foot-in-thedoor condition, individuals were first asked to sign a petition in support of the museum; (2) in the door-in-the-face condition, they were initially asked for a $5 contribution; (3) in the. low-ball condition, they were asked to contribute 75 cents and then to increase this amount by 25 cents in order to support the children's program: and (4) in the control condition, subjects simply received the target request for $1. The groups differed significantly in terms of the number of individuals who donated, and in the amount of their contributions. The low-ball condition was generally the most effective, especially with respect to the amount of money contributed. This was followed in turn by the door-in-the-face and control conditions, with the foot-in-the-door condition the least effective in virtually every comparison. These findings are discussed in terms of their implications for the experimental analysis of compliance, as well as the development of more effective fund-raising programs. 相似文献
220.