首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   72841篇
  免费   2942篇
  国内免费   22篇
  2020年   695篇
  2019年   951篇
  2018年   1225篇
  2017年   1254篇
  2016年   1341篇
  2015年   951篇
  2014年   1183篇
  2013年   5534篇
  2012年   2125篇
  2011年   2348篇
  2010年   1449篇
  2009年   1450篇
  2008年   2066篇
  2007年   2077篇
  2006年   1940篇
  2005年   1674篇
  2004年   1583篇
  2003年   1489篇
  2002年   1581篇
  2001年   2274篇
  2000年   2300篇
  1999年   1718篇
  1998年   862篇
  1997年   750篇
  1996年   754篇
  1995年   671篇
  1992年   1444篇
  1991年   1346篇
  1990年   1320篇
  1989年   1220篇
  1988年   1203篇
  1987年   1132篇
  1986年   1224篇
  1985年   1246篇
  1984年   1034篇
  1983年   961篇
  1982年   702篇
  1981年   696篇
  1979年   1129篇
  1978年   843篇
  1975年   923篇
  1974年   1008篇
  1973年   1111篇
  1972年   934篇
  1971年   877篇
  1970年   761篇
  1969年   820篇
  1968年   1040篇
  1967年   959篇
  1966年   806篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
891.
Sternheim and Boynton’s (1966) continuous judgmental color-naming technique was used to assess the elemental nature of achromatic colors. Results from three subjects indicate that the names “black” and “white” are necessary and sufficient to describe achromatic test lights associated with the name “gray.” On the basis of the criteria that establishes a color as elemental, black and white were confirmed as elemental, whereas gray was not. These findings support the assumption of various opponent processing models that there are only two elemental achromatic sensations. In addition, the results support Hering’s (1920) view that black and white differ from the paired-chromatic opponent colors in that they are not mutually exclusive.  相似文献   
892.
Two experiments investigated infants’ sensitivity to familiar size as information for the distances of objects with which they had had only brief experience. Each experiment had two phases: a familiarization phase and a test phase. During the familiarization phase, the infant played with a pair of different-sized objects for 10 min. During the test phase, a pair of objects, identical to those seen in the familiarization phase but now equal in size, were presented to the infant at a fixed distance under monocular or binocular viewing conditions. In the test phase of Experiment 1, 7-month-old infants viewing the objects monocularly showed a significant preference to reach for the object that resembled the smaller object in the familiarization phase. Seven-month-old infants in the binocular viewing condition reached equally to the two test phase objects. These results indicate that, in the monocular condition, the 7-month-olds used knowledge about the objects’ sizes, acquired during the familiarization phase, to perceive distance from the test objects’ visual angles, and that they reached preferentially for the apparently nearer object. The lack of a reaching preference in the binocular condition rules out interpretations of the results not based on the objects’ perceived distances. The results, therefore, indicate that 7-month-old infants can use memory to mediate spatial perception. The implications of this finding for the debate between direct and indirect theories of visual perception are discussed. In the test phase of Experiment 2,5-month-old infants viewing the objects monocularly showed no reaching preference. These infants, therefore, showed no evidence of sensitivity to familiar size as distance information.  相似文献   
893.
Syntactic determinants of sentence comprehension in aphasia   总被引:5,自引:0,他引:5  
D Caplan  C Baker  F Dehaut 《Cognition》1985,21(2):117-175
  相似文献   
894.
895.
Four experiments examined the memory processes used to maintain location in a counting sequence. In Experiment 1, subjects who rapidly counted forward omitted many repeated-digit numbers (e.g., 77), as found previously with backward counting. Subjects in Experiment 2 counted backward with normal auditory feedback or with headphones through which white noise was channeled. In both cases, repeated-digit errors predominated, suggesting that the contents of short-term memory, rather than auditory sensory memory, are checked during counting. In Experiment 3, subjects silently wrote counting responses, and the omission errors resembled those in vocal counting. Repetition errors were also found and attributed to phonological recoding failures. Articulatory suppression in Experiment 4 greatly increased the number of repetition errors in the written counting task. A model of the counting process was proposed according to which subjects keep track of their location in the counting sequence by monitoring phonologically coded short-term memory representations of the numbers.  相似文献   
896.
897.
A small informal poll of local colleagues revealed that despite their interest in the subject, many had doubts about the possibility of analyzing persons aged sixty or over successfully. Their specific questions are collated and are offered as a sample of the questions in the minds of many analysts everywhere. Selected material from the ongoing analysis of a sixty-four-year-old patient is presented that bears on some of the questions raised. In addition, changes in the patient are noted. These are discussed in terms of changes in function, which are equated with structural change. The major thrust of the paper is that development continues throughout the life course, and that the criteria for analyzability remain constant. The chronological age of the adult by itself is not an indicator of eligibility or of suitability for psychoanalysis.  相似文献   
898.
899.
900.
Dieting and binging. A causal analysis   总被引:16,自引:0,他引:16  
  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号