全文获取类型
收费全文 | 87506篇 |
免费 | 1732篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
89241篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 825篇 |
2019年 | 1112篇 |
2018年 | 1436篇 |
2017年 | 1489篇 |
2016年 | 1578篇 |
2015年 | 1145篇 |
2014年 | 1380篇 |
2013年 | 6331篇 |
2012年 | 2534篇 |
2011年 | 2724篇 |
2010年 | 1703篇 |
2009年 | 1673篇 |
2008年 | 2413篇 |
2007年 | 2407篇 |
2006年 | 2208篇 |
2005年 | 1972篇 |
2004年 | 1859篇 |
2003年 | 1735篇 |
2002年 | 1869篇 |
2001年 | 2680篇 |
2000年 | 2576篇 |
1999年 | 1971篇 |
1998年 | 1011篇 |
1997年 | 881篇 |
1996年 | 876篇 |
1992年 | 1633篇 |
1991年 | 1532篇 |
1990年 | 1523篇 |
1989年 | 1432篇 |
1988年 | 1410篇 |
1987年 | 1339篇 |
1986年 | 1417篇 |
1985年 | 1482篇 |
1984年 | 1236篇 |
1983年 | 1149篇 |
1982年 | 864篇 |
1981年 | 855篇 |
1979年 | 1352篇 |
1978年 | 967篇 |
1976年 | 816篇 |
1975年 | 1107篇 |
1974年 | 1205篇 |
1973年 | 1295篇 |
1972年 | 1076篇 |
1971年 | 1028篇 |
1970年 | 940篇 |
1969年 | 989篇 |
1968年 | 1233篇 |
1967年 | 1106篇 |
1966年 | 975篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
111.
When thinking under uncertainty, people often do not consider appropriately each of the relevant branches of a decision tree, as required by consequentialism. As a result they sometimes violate Savage's sure-thing principle. In the Prisoner's Dilemma game, for example, many subjects compete when they know that the opponent has competed and when they know that the opponent has cooperated, but cooperate when they do not know the opponent's response. Newcomb's Problem and Wason's selection task are also interpreted as manifestations of nonconsequential decision making and reasoning. The causes and implications of such behavior, and the notion of quasi-magical thinking, are discussed. 相似文献
112.
Light and Humphreys (1981, Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 31, 521-530) provided evidence that young children's drawings, despite infrequently showing view-specific occlusion, do systematically reflect spatial relations within an array. The present research tested the hypothesis that young children's preferences for canonical "best views" interact with array-faithful tendencies to increase early uses of occlusion. Forty-three children between 4 and 7 years of age drew arrays like Light and Humphreys' end-to-end alignments, with end-on views of objects in depth, and arrays aligned side-to-side, with canonical side-views of objects in depth. Significantly fewer single-object, view-specific occlusions were produced for end-to-end than for side-to-side alignments. Nevertheless, the former reveal that more children are able to use the vertical dimension to depict multiple objects in depth. Other comparisons suggest an interaction in multiple-object depictions of canonicality with spatial dimension and graphic complexity. 相似文献
113.
J Duffy 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1992,53(2):151-158
A study was made of the choice reaction times of 8-, 10-, 12-year-olds and adults in order to investigate whether length information stored in memory was coded to preserve numerical relations between stimuli. Subjects learned unique line length-color associations and were later presented with pairs of colors and were instructed to choose the color that had been associated with the longer line. For all age groups, choice reaction times were related to both differences and ratios. These results were interpreted as supporting the analog theory of representation. 相似文献
114.
Three mirror tracing experiments were conducted to investigate the connection between perception and motor behavior. In the first experiment, some subjects traced a hex-maze, other subjects traced a hex-maze after observing a model trace, others traced a hex-maze after reading instructions on mirror images, and others traced a hex-maze after having observed a model and heard the instructions. There were no significant differences between the groups' error scores, but their time scores differed significantly, although not always in the predicted direction. In Experiment 2, the subjects were to trace selected letters of the alphabet. Error scores for the second experiment did not differ much from those for the first experiment. In Experiment 3, the experimenter gave each subject commands for the correct directions of movement, using the subject's body as a frame-of-reference. There was little improvement in motor performance. These results suggest that the visual information presented in the mirror captured the subjects' attention and blocked their motor tracing program. 相似文献
115.
Associations of two different response sets on the Infrequent Response Index of the Strong Interest Inventory (SII) were examined. In one, participants responded to each item randomly; in the other, participants deliberately misrepresented their responses. The finding of scores higher than those proposed in the SII manual for the Infrequent Response Index suggests an alternative interpretation of this critical index. 相似文献
116.
This study examined the career aspirations of pregnant teens enrolled in a program designed to minimize their chances of dropping out of high school. The teens completed a demographic data form that included questions on their career aspirations. The data were coded by Holland type using the Dictionary of Holland Occupational Codes. Trends were discussed in light of the future labor market and critical need for career counseling for this at-risk population. 相似文献
117.
The intent of this study is to apply a cross-level design to examine the influence of contextual factors upon individuals' performance. The results support the hypothesis that peer group characteristics predict individual performance beyond individual characteristics. The implications for using a cross-level approach to study the effects of contextual variables on individual-level phenomena are discussed. 相似文献
118.
This article addresses the relationship between religious affiliation and a very specific form of firearms ownership, that is, handgun ownership. Literature is reviewed relative to explanations of gun ownership. A test of the hypothesized relationship between religious affiliation and handgun ownership demonstrated a statistically significant ( p < 0.03) association. Protestants were found to have a disproportionately high level of handgun ownership compared to other religious groups. Speculation for this finding is reviewed relative to other recent research on this topic. 相似文献
119.
David C. Rowe Laurie Chassin Clark C. Presson Debra Edwards Steven J. Sherman 《Journal of applied social psychology》1992,22(4):261-285
The prevalence of regular smoking increases rapidly during adolescence. We applied modeling methods from the study of epidemic infectious disease to smoking behavior. Our "epidemic" models dealt with the process of transition among four states: nonsmoker (susceptible), trier (exposed), regular smoker (infected), and ex-smoker (recovered). The best fitting model was one in which the transition from nonsmoker to trier was a social contagion process where nonsmokers contacted either triers or regular smokers. All other transitions were treated as constant rate processes in which a constant proportion of eligible individuals made the transition in a given year. The recursive equation model embodying these assumptions was able to predict accurately the proportion of adolescents in each state in Grades 6–11. Our results support a "stage" model of smoking, suggesting that different psychological and physiological processes are involved in the initiation of smoking than in developing a habit of regular smoking. A possible sex difference was found in the non-contact transition between trier and smoker states, with the transition being more probable in females than in males. 相似文献
120.
D E Schotte 《Journal of personality and social psychology》1992,62(5):798-800
This article comments on a recent article by Heatherton, Herman, and Polivy (1991) in which they suggest that ego threats are a prerequisite to affectively induced disinhibition of food intake in restrained eaters and current dieters. In contrast, the present review suggests that mood induction procedures that involve no apparent threat (e.g., viewing a frightening film) can also prompt disinhibition in restrained eaters. Thus, any mood induction procedure that does not directly physically threaten the S may disinhibit restrained eaters. It is concluded that recommendations to focus future research on ego threats are premature and may serve to obscure the mechanisms by which changes in affective state influence food intake in dieters. 相似文献