全文获取类型
收费全文 | 83331篇 |
免费 | 3411篇 |
国内免费 | 38篇 |
专业分类
86780篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 839篇 |
2019年 | 1095篇 |
2018年 | 1475篇 |
2017年 | 1478篇 |
2016年 | 1611篇 |
2015年 | 1147篇 |
2014年 | 1365篇 |
2013年 | 6282篇 |
2012年 | 2534篇 |
2011年 | 2683篇 |
2010年 | 1642篇 |
2009年 | 1689篇 |
2008年 | 2327篇 |
2007年 | 2373篇 |
2006年 | 2151篇 |
2005年 | 1909篇 |
2004年 | 1787篇 |
2003年 | 1675篇 |
2002年 | 1780篇 |
2001年 | 2685篇 |
2000年 | 2651篇 |
1999年 | 1994篇 |
1998年 | 939篇 |
1997年 | 849篇 |
1996年 | 838篇 |
1995年 | 796篇 |
1993年 | 772篇 |
1992年 | 1668篇 |
1991年 | 1534篇 |
1990年 | 1520篇 |
1989年 | 1408篇 |
1988年 | 1395篇 |
1987年 | 1311篇 |
1986年 | 1375篇 |
1985年 | 1435篇 |
1984年 | 1201篇 |
1983年 | 1101篇 |
1982年 | 812篇 |
1979年 | 1280篇 |
1978年 | 925篇 |
1975年 | 1026篇 |
1974年 | 1152篇 |
1973年 | 1263篇 |
1972年 | 1081篇 |
1971年 | 987篇 |
1970年 | 886篇 |
1969年 | 925篇 |
1968年 | 1174篇 |
1967年 | 1061篇 |
1966年 | 925篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
971.
Awareness of the response after feedback training for changes in heart rate and sudomotor laterality
L E Roberts R J Williams R G Marlin T Farrell D Imiolo 《Journal of experimental psychology. General》1984,113(2):225-255
What is the relation between the ability to control visceral responding on a biofeedback task and the ability to report behaviors actually contributing to this performance? Subjects received biofeedback training for unidentified visceral responses and then gave written reports about what they had done to control the feedback displays. Independent judges were given these reports and, on the basis of knowledge about activities known to contribute to visceral activity, were asked to determine the visceral responses for which the subjects had been trained. The reports of subjects who succeeded at bidirectional control of heart rate (Experiment 1) or sudomotor laterality (Experiment 2) showed awareness of behaviors related to feedback as assessed by this procedure, whereas the reports of subjects who failed at bidirectional control did not. Subsequent experiments indicated that these results did not depend on a learning strategy that might have been specific to the initial studies. These findings call into question the view that people are unaware of what they have done to produce the response after training on biofeedback tasks. Earlier studies reporting lack of awareness in biofeedback are discussed in light of factors that affect the measurement of biofeedback learning and response awareness. 相似文献
972.
R A Rosellini J P DeCola M Plonsky D A Warren A J Stilman 《Journal of experimental psychology. Animal behavior processes》1984,10(3):346-359
Learned helplessness theory predicts that animals exposed to inescapable shock acquire an expectancy of response-reinforcer independence, which proactively interferes with learning of response-reinforcer dependence. The theory also predicts that this expectancy can increase sensitivity to subsequent instances of response-reinforcer independence. These experiments test the latter prediction in a paradigm that minimizes the confounding effects of shock-induced activity deficits. Rats were trained to respond for food, then given either escapable, inescapable, or no shock. Subsequently, they received two sessions of response-contingent food followed by sessions of noncontingent food deliveries. During this phase, inescapably shocked animals decreased responding faster than did controls. Experiment 2 replicated this finding with a different schedule of food delivery and a procedure that more directly minimized the possibility that the outcome is due to either direct or indirect shock-induced activity changes. These results support the prediction that uncontrollable aversive events can increase an animal's sensitivity to noncontingent response-reinforcer relationships. 相似文献
973.
A battery of information processing measures and psychometric tests of specific and general cognitive abilities was administered to a sample of 105 individuals which included 34 monozygotic and 13 dizygotic reared apart twin pairs or triplets. Correlations between information processing parameters and psychometric abilities as well as twin resemblances for the information processing parameters were examined. In a principal components analysis of the information processing parameters, three components were identified which accounted for 67% of the total variation; Overall Speed of Response (OSR), Speed of Information Processing (SIP), and Speed of Spatial Processing (SSP). OSR was significantly correlated with WAIS IQ, and psychometric measures of verbal reasoning, spatial ability, and perceptual speed and accuracy. SIP was significantly correlated with WAIS IQ, measures of verbal reasoning, and one of the perceptual speed and accuracy measures. SSP was significantly correlated with measures of both spatial ability and perceptual speed and accuracy. Due to the small size of the dizygotic twin sample, no strong conclusions could be drawn regarding the magnitude of their resemblance. The correlation between component scores of reared apart monozygotic twins was significant for OSR, but not for SIP or SSP. 相似文献
974.
WILLIAM SHREEVE MURIEL R. RADEBAUGH JANET R. NORBY WILLIAM G. J. GOETTER T. K. MIDGLEY ARNOLD F. STUECKLE BARBARA DE MICHELE 《Counseling and values》1984,29(1):59-66
Although the use of merit pay, incentive pay, bonus pay, and job promotion are well-established measures in the private or corporate sector of American society, few examples of successful teacher merit pay systems exist. In 1983, the Eastern Washington University Department of Education surveyed six major private corporations to determine whether corporate merit pay programs could be applied to teachers and educational institutions. Survey findings suggest several reasons why merit pay programs for teachers will probably fail: (a) lack of monetary goals; (b) the necessity for subjective evaluation, which requires multiple measurement devices, extensive supervision, and time; (c) the absence of a many-tiered corporate structure in most educational institutions; and (d) increased supervisory direction that could stifle creativity and flexibility. 相似文献
975.
Subjects were presented with a sequence of picture-word items. The pictures were line drawings of common objects, and the words were the names of the objects. Half of the items were followed quickly by the next item, and the other half by a 15-sec “rehearsal interval.” The subjects were told to use these intervals to rehearse the item just presented, either by saying the word over and over to themselves or by maintaining the picture before the mind’s eye. There followed a test in which each item from the study list was cued with a fragment of either the picture or the name. Consistent with previous findings, identification was more probable for fragments of items that had been followed by a rehearsal interval. In addition, this advantage was found to be greater when the type of cue matched the mode of rehearsal that the subject had been instructed to adopt; there was in fact little, if any, advantage of rehearsal when cue and mode of rehearsal mismatched. This pattern of results suggests functionally distinct pictorial and verbal modes of rehearsal that serve not only to maintain information in conscious mind but also to build up memory proper. 相似文献
976.
977.
Memory & Cognition - In two experiments, children were shown a series of pictures of common objects and were asked to free recall which pictured objects had been presented. Some pictures were... 相似文献
978.
The effect of environmental enrichment on normal rats, rats that received hemidecortication at birth, and rats that received hemidecortication when adult was studied in the Morris water task, which is a spatial navigation task requiring the use of distal cues for successful performance. Enrichment was achieved by housing the rats for 90 days in a large outdoor compound that was designed to model a natural environment. Adult hemidecorticated lab-raised rats were severely impaired in the acquisition of the task but neonatally hemidecorticated rats, although also impaired, showed significant sparing. Postsurgical, but not presurgical, enrichment of adult hemidecorticated rats significantly enhanced their recovery. Enrichment had little effect on neonatally decorticated rats or on normal rats. Although there were no gross anatomical changes associated with enrichment, the paradigm provides both a robust phenomena and a cortically dependent task that is ideal for investigating the processes in the remaining intact hemisphere that must support recovery. 相似文献
979.
Patrick C. L. Heaven 《Personality and individual differences》1984,5(6):741-742
Previous research among university students has identified four classes of assertive behavior, namely, Directiveness, Social Assertiveness, Defense of One's Rights and Interests and Independence. Scores on the Lorr and More (1980) Assertiveness Inventory, obtained from a general population sample, were subjected to Varimax rotatio. Results revealed only three classes of assertiveness. It is argued that Directiveness may be inferred from various forms of assertiveness, rather than being a separate and distinct class of assertive behavior. 相似文献
980.