首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   17180篇
  免费   810篇
  国内免费   15篇
  18005篇
  2021年   136篇
  2020年   234篇
  2019年   250篇
  2018年   364篇
  2017年   370篇
  2016年   371篇
  2015年   285篇
  2014年   312篇
  2013年   1353篇
  2012年   578篇
  2011年   653篇
  2010年   380篇
  2009年   352篇
  2008年   485篇
  2007年   465篇
  2006年   406篇
  2005年   348篇
  2004年   350篇
  2003年   332篇
  2002年   334篇
  2001年   705篇
  2000年   701篇
  1999年   463篇
  1998年   182篇
  1997年   179篇
  1996年   142篇
  1995年   140篇
  1993年   142篇
  1992年   364篇
  1991年   323篇
  1990年   349篇
  1989年   338篇
  1988年   323篇
  1987年   291篇
  1986年   313篇
  1985年   301篇
  1984年   238篇
  1983年   201篇
  1979年   191篇
  1978年   164篇
  1975年   189篇
  1974年   242篇
  1973年   201篇
  1972年   203篇
  1971年   168篇
  1970年   149篇
  1969年   200篇
  1968年   204篇
  1967年   162篇
  1966年   184篇
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
181.
One of the significant limitations of the motor control and skill acquisition domain is that the theories, models, and hypotheses are, in most cases, task specific. Many lines of theorizing fail to hold up under even small changes in task constraints, although clearly the field does have some robust phenomena. It is proposed that a broader consideration of the role of task constraints, which is grounded in the methodology of nonlinear dynamics, may help to formulate a more general action theory of coordination and control.  相似文献   
182.
183.
Two groups of American undergraduates (moderate and heavy social drinkers) completed a matrix task and received either positive or negative feedback on their performance. Following this they were given a maze task, which was designed so that cheating could be detected. Heavy drinkers cheated more than moderate drinkers under success conditions (positive feedback). Heavy drinkers who received positive feedback also cheated more than heavy drinkers who received negative feedback. The results are interpreted in terms of self-handicapping theory.  相似文献   
184.
We provided tactile cues to a student who was deaf, blind, and mentally retarded to guide her performance on a variety of packaging tasks. The student had previously received extensive training on multiple packaging and sequencing tasks through her vocational education program. Although she was able to complete these tasks, each change in materials necessitated that similar levels of retraining be conducted in order for her to perform revised tasks. Tactile cues were introduced and evaluated through a multiple baseline with sequential withdrawal design for two envelope-stuffing tasks and one bagging task. Results indicated that the tactile prompts were effective in guiding her performance on the training task and in promoting generalization to novel tasks and cues. Continued use of the cues was necessary to maintain the student's performance. Our findings suggest that tactile prompts function similarly to picture prompts and may be an effective alternative external prompting system for persons for whom picture prompts would not be appropriate.  相似文献   
185.
186.
Hypnotic hallucination alters evoked potentials   总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2  
  相似文献   
187.
188.
189.
Two experiments assessed the importance of sound duration for eliciting head orientation responses from newborn infants. In Experiment 1, thirty infants turned with equal frequency toward 20-s continuous rattle sounds and 20-s trains of rattle segments. The duration of the rattle segments--14 and 100 ms (2/s), or 500 ms (1/s)--did not influence the likelihood of turning. Response latencies and durations proved quite similar for all stimuli. In Experiment 2, twenty-four infants heard continuous rattle sounds of four different durations: 1, 5, 10, and 20 s. They turned reliably to all stimulus durations; furthermore, the magnitude and temporal characteristics of head orientation responses did not differ for the four stimulus durations. These results suggest that the newborn's head orientation response may reflect a motor program that is initiated by auditory input and then executed in a similar fashion regardless of further stimulation.  相似文献   
190.
Sex roles, gender, and fear   总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1  
Male and female college students classified as masculine, feminine, androgynous, or undifferentiated on the Bem (1974) Sex Role Inventory (N = 199) were given the Wolpe (1969) Fear Inventory. Significant differences in average fear scores were seen between men and women and between the four sex-role categories, with sex role and gender contributing equally to the variability. The argument is raised here that excessive fearfulness or fearlessness might be as much a product of sex-role conditioning as a product of gender.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号