首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   3170篇
  免费   179篇
  2023年   37篇
  2022年   63篇
  2021年   87篇
  2020年   121篇
  2019年   98篇
  2018年   187篇
  2017年   193篇
  2016年   161篇
  2015年   89篇
  2014年   119篇
  2013年   425篇
  2012年   238篇
  2011年   239篇
  2010年   152篇
  2009年   77篇
  2008年   195篇
  2007年   144篇
  2006年   129篇
  2005年   71篇
  2004年   61篇
  2003年   67篇
  2002年   46篇
  2001年   30篇
  2000年   38篇
  1999年   22篇
  1998年   20篇
  1997年   15篇
  1996年   7篇
  1995年   7篇
  1994年   10篇
  1993年   11篇
  1992年   6篇
  1991年   19篇
  1990年   8篇
  1989年   11篇
  1988年   10篇
  1987年   10篇
  1986年   11篇
  1985年   12篇
  1984年   6篇
  1982年   6篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   5篇
  1979年   6篇
  1972年   5篇
  1971年   7篇
  1968年   10篇
  1967年   5篇
  1966年   7篇
  1965年   8篇
排序方式: 共有3349条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
981.
Journal of Religion and Health - Using a representative sample, we explored the relationship of organizational (ORA), non-organizational (NORA), and intrinsic religiosity (IR) with depressive and...  相似文献   
982.
Brand names are often considered a special type of words of special relevance to examine the role of visual codes during reading: unlike common words, brand names are typically presented with the same letter-case configuration (e.g., IKEA, adidas). Recently, Pathak et al. (European Journal of Marketing, 2019, 53 , 2109) found an effect of visual similarity for misspelled brand names when the participants had to decide whether the brand name was spelled correctly or not (e.g., tacebook [baseword: facebook] was responded more slowly and less accurately than xacebook). This finding is at odds with both orthographically based visual-word recognition models and prior experiments using misspelled common words (e.g., viotin [baseword: violin ] is identified as fast as viocin ). To solve this puzzle, we designed two experiments in which the participants had to decide whether the presented item was written correctly. In Experiment 1, following a procedure similar to Pathak et al. (European Journal of Marketing, 2019, 53 , 2109), we examined the effect of visual similarity on misspelled brand names with/without graphical information (e.g., anazon vs. atazon [baseword: amazon ]). Experiment 2 was parallel to Experiment 1, but we focused on misspelled common words (e.g., anarillo vs. atarillo ; baseword: amarillo [yellow in Spanish]). Results showed a sizeable effect of visual similarity on misspelled brand names – regardless of their graphical information, but not on misspelled common words. These findings suggest that visual codes play a greater role when identifying brand names than common words. We examined how models of visual-word recognition can account for this dissociation.  相似文献   
983.
Jiménez-Pazos  Bárbara 《Topoi》2022,41(3):561-571

The aesthetically optimistic view of life in the last paragraph of Darwin’s On the Origin of Species contrasts with the evidence in his autobiography of a supposed perceptive colour blindness to the magnificence of nature. Accepting the theory of evolution as one of the scientific theories that has contributed to disenchantment, my aim is to delve into the Darwinian perception of natural beauty and solve this contrast of perceptions within the framework of the Weberian concept of “disenchantment of the world.” To do this, I have carried out a computer-assisted study of Darwin’s language focusing on the frequency of aesthetic-emotional and religious adjectives and adverbs in the six editions of the Origin. A semantic analysis of the lexicon shows that, although disenchanted, Darwin perceives nature as aesthetically enhanced. I arrive at the conclusion that Darwin’s alleged colour blindness does not come from a loss of his capacity to aesthetically perceive nature, but from his loss of religious belief.

  相似文献   
984.
Journal of Clinical Psychology in Medical Settings - This study aimed to examine the role of decentering and committed action as mediators of the link of external and internal shame with...  相似文献   
985.
Journal of Child and Family Studies - This qualitative case study describes three adult siblings’ experiences and their perceptions of support connected with the time before and after their...  相似文献   
986.
987.
Phenomenology and the Cognitive Sciences - The present paper explores the phenomenology of disorientation and its relationship with self-consciousness. Section 1 discusses previous literature on...  相似文献   
988.
Applied Research in Quality of Life - This research study tested an intervention, based on autonomous teams and the service co-creation approach, to improve the social inclusion and...  相似文献   
989.
Applied Research in Quality of Life - A Correction to this paper has been published: https://doi.org/10.1007/s11482-021-09935-5  相似文献   
990.
The perceived global motion of a stimulus depends on how its different local motion-direction vectors are distributed in space and time. When they are explicitly co-localized, as in the case of locally paired motion, competitive motion integration mechanisms produce a unitary global motion direction determined by their vector average. During motion aftereffects induced by simultaneous adaptation to multiple motion directions, just as in the case of locally paired motion, different directional signals originate simultaneously from exactly the same position in space. Therefore, the perceived global motion direction during motion aftereffects results from local vector averaging of the co-localized motion-direction signals induced by adaptation.  相似文献   
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号