全文获取类型
收费全文 | 4447篇 |
免费 | 61篇 |
专业分类
4508篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 42篇 |
2023年 | 58篇 |
2022年 | 86篇 |
2021年 | 110篇 |
2020年 | 149篇 |
2019年 | 152篇 |
2018年 | 218篇 |
2017年 | 234篇 |
2016年 | 184篇 |
2015年 | 121篇 |
2014年 | 134篇 |
2013年 | 552篇 |
2012年 | 310篇 |
2011年 | 310篇 |
2010年 | 196篇 |
2009年 | 114篇 |
2008年 | 263篇 |
2007年 | 207篇 |
2006年 | 184篇 |
2005年 | 119篇 |
2004年 | 97篇 |
2003年 | 96篇 |
2002年 | 72篇 |
2001年 | 42篇 |
2000年 | 54篇 |
1999年 | 36篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 20篇 |
1996年 | 13篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 14篇 |
1993年 | 13篇 |
1992年 | 15篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 18篇 |
1988年 | 18篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 13篇 |
1985年 | 14篇 |
1984年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 12篇 |
1980年 | 8篇 |
1979年 | 10篇 |
1972年 | 10篇 |
1971年 | 8篇 |
1968年 | 9篇 |
1966年 | 8篇 |
1965年 | 10篇 |
排序方式: 共有4508条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
141.
This work presents a critical analysis of Pavlov's influence that goes beyond the conventional view: that which reduces his influence in American psychology to the behaviorism of Watson and Hull. In order to understand the nature of the Russian physiologist's influence in American psychology, we propose a distinction between three approaches to it: 1) the symbolic approach, on representing a model of the possibility of constructing an objective psychology; 2) the methodological approach, given the importance of the technique of conditional reflexes; and 3) the theoretical approach, which is derived from his theory of higher nervous activity. This perspective permits us to suggest that most of Pavlov's influence on behaviorism was of a symbolic and methodological nature--though the methodological influence also reached other authors that did not belong to the behaviorist traditions, as was the case of Mateer. As far as the theoretical influence is concerned, our work proposes that it is more visible in authors such as Gantt and Liddell, or even in authors such as Boldirev, Director of the Pavlovian Laboratory at the Battle Creek Sanitarium in Michigan. The case of Gantt is especially interesting because, in addition to his important contributions, he played an essential role in the foundation of the Pavlovian Society, and the journal Conditional Reflex. What our work proposes is that to understand the nature of Pavlov's influence in American psychology it is necessary to take into account the very characteristics of that psychology: its pragmatic interests, its methodological rigor, the dominant systems of neo-behavioral theory and the changes that occurred after the Second World War. 相似文献
142.
Studies in East European Thought - 相似文献
143.
This study was aimed at the examination of ‘mental construction’ in paradise fish by interactive learning, which is suggested
as an alternative hypothesis for backward or second-order conditioning. Avoidance of goldfish was established in paradise
fish by presenting a harmless goldfish (a novel stimulus) after an aversive event (mild electric shocks) in the dark compartment
of a shuttle tank. It was found that this avoidance depended on context pre-exposure. Experiment 1 was designed to study the
effect of mild shocks on shuttling activity in a familiar context. Experiment 2 aimed at establishing fear-conditioning to
the goldfish in a higher-order conditioning paradigm. In the course of training, unpaired stimuli were presented in the dark
compartment of the shuttle tank in such a manner that the presentation of mild shocks (20 mA) preceded the encounter with
a harmless fish (goldfish). Experiment 3 demonstrated the role of context pre-exposure in interactive learning. Results indicate
that (1) while 60 mA shocks resulted in avoidance of the dark compartment, the 20 mA mild shocks affected exploratory behaviour;
(2) after pre-exposure to the training environment, paradise fish avoided the dark compartment containing goldfish provided
that subjects had previously encountered mild, explorative shocks there; (3) this conditioned fear, a ‘mental construction’
of a potential danger, was proved to be transferable to another context and was consequently aimed specifically at the goldfish,
a living being, rather than the training context; (4) the pre-exposure to the shuttle tank was an important part of this training
procedure, that is, only subjects habituated to the shuttle tank avoided the goldfish. Results are discussed in the framework
of the Interactive Learning Hypothesis, which has been developed as an ethological approach to a higher-order conditioning
paradigm.
Received: 15 January 1999 / Accepted after revision: 18 August 1999 相似文献
144.
Réka Török István Tóth-Király Beáta Bőthe Gábor Orosz 《Current psychology (New Brunswick, N.J.)》2017,36(4):764-773
The goal of the study was to examine the dimensionality of the Career Decision Self-Efficacy Scale Short Form (CDSES-SF, Betz et al. 1996). Integrating several previous findings from different cultures, we found that the bifactor structure of the CDSES-SF on a Hungarian sample of 649 respondents showed the best model fit. This structure includes a general CDSE factor covering 15 items and the original five specific factors (self-appraisal, occupational information, goal selection, planning, and problem solving) covering three items each. This short form of CDSES shows an acceptable model fit and appropriate reliability in terms of the Cronbach’s alpha and omega values. Regarding career decision self-efficacy, a large proportion of variance was explained by the general factor and to a smaller extent by the specific factors. These results can be considered as a first step in resolving the paradox of the dimensionality of CDSES-SF. 相似文献
145.
146.
Three studies demonstrate a novel phenomenon--self-regulatory outsourcing--in which thinking about how other people can be instrumental (i.e., helpful) for a given goal undermines motivation to expend effort on that goal. In Experiment 1, participants who thought about how their partner helped them with health goals (as opposed to career goals) planned to spend less time and effort on health goals in the upcoming week. This pattern was stronger for depleted participants than for nondepleted participants. In Experiment 2, participants who thought about how their partner helped them with academic-achievement goals procrastinated more, leaving themselves less time for an academic task, than did participants in two control conditions. This pattern was stronger for participants who were told that procrastinating would drain their resources for the academic task than for participants who were told that procrastinating would not drain their resources for that task. In Experiment 3, participants who decreased their effort after thinking of an instrumental significant other reported higher relationship commitment to that individual than did participants who did not decrease their effort. The possibility for shared (or transactive) self-regulation is discussed. 相似文献
147.
The possibility that rats can navigate in the Morris water maze by reducing the difference between the memorized platform scene and the current sensory input was tested in nine blind rats. A computerized videosystem monitored the rats' movements in the pool and converted the rat-target distance into tones the frequency of which increased in 64 equal steps from 120 Hz at 128 cm to 7680 Hz at 0 cm. During 15 days of training to find a fixed platform position from different starting points (12 trials per day) average escape latencies decreased from 39.0 to 25.4 s. The performance significantly deteriorated when the acoustic distance signalization was omitted and/or when the target position was changed form trial to trial. It is concluded that blind rats solved the task by simultaneously employing search strategy based on position responses, mapping using acoustic background beacons, and distance reduction navigation. It is argued that the various strategies are additive and that their relative significance depends of the conditions of the experiment. 相似文献
148.
The present research examines the semantic priming effects of a centrally presented single prime word to which participants were instructed to either "attend and remember" or "ignore". The prime word was followed by a central probe target on which the participants made a lexical decision task. The main variables manipulated across experiments were prime duration (50 or 100 ms), the presence or absence of a mask following the prime, and the presence (or absence) and type of distractor stimulus (random set of consonants or pseudowords) on the probe display. There was a consistent interaction between the instructions and the semantic priming effects. Relative to the "attend and remember" instruction, an "ignore" instruction produced reduced positive priming from single primes presented for 100 ms, irrespective of the presence or absence of a prime mask, and regardless of whether the probe target was presented with or without distractors. Additionally, reliable negative priming was found from ignored primes presented for briefer durations (50 ms) and immediately followed by a mask. Methodological and theoretical implications of the present findings for the extant negative priming literature are discussed. 相似文献
149.
César González 《Sexuality & culture》2007,11(4):31-47
This article focuses on the relevance of age vis-à-vis the body and sexuality in society. The author discusses how the symbolization
of the body by differences of age displays an intertwining of social and sexuality issues. The central idea is that the constructions
of age involve different notions of status related to the ageing body that are problematic in our sexuality because they form
complex power relationships, creating boundaries, segmenting sexual expressions and committing individuals to spaces, identities
and lifestyles. That is to say, as individuals use methods to capitalize on their sexuality, they produce varying meanings
and ways of experiencing age. The notion of age-grading, an anthropological category to explain the connection between the
notions of ageing body and society, will be discussed with the aim of clarifying how age shapes our sexuality. This paper
is meant to encourage further debate, not to provide a finished discussion. 相似文献
150.
Simón A 《Psychological reports》2007,101(2):482-484
The shortened versions of the 16 PF (Forms C or D) were given to 227 college students (who took Form D twice), 33 prisoners (who took Forms C and D), and 95 substance abusers (who took Form C twice). All were asked to answer honestly, then retake these tests to appear in a better light. Significant differences in Motivational Distortion scores were found between test and retest in each group. 相似文献