全文获取类型
收费全文 | 501篇 |
免费 | 32篇 |
专业分类
533篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 3篇 |
2023年 | 10篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 10篇 |
2020年 | 8篇 |
2019年 | 20篇 |
2018年 | 23篇 |
2017年 | 17篇 |
2016年 | 29篇 |
2015年 | 19篇 |
2014年 | 32篇 |
2013年 | 59篇 |
2012年 | 36篇 |
2011年 | 33篇 |
2010年 | 30篇 |
2009年 | 20篇 |
2008年 | 34篇 |
2007年 | 25篇 |
2006年 | 21篇 |
2005年 | 18篇 |
2004年 | 15篇 |
2003年 | 12篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 5篇 |
2000年 | 3篇 |
1999年 | 1篇 |
1998年 | 7篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1995年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 4篇 |
1993年 | 2篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 2篇 |
1990年 | 2篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1987年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 2篇 |
1982年 | 2篇 |
1981年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 1篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 1篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有533条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
Isabel Gauthier Kao-Wei Chua Jennifer J. Richler 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2018,80(6):1449-1460
The Vanderbilt Holistic Processing Test for faces (VHPT-F) is the first standard test designed to measure individual differences in holistic processing. The test measures failures of selective attention to face parts through congruency effects, an operational definition of holistic processing. However, this conception of holistic processing has been challenged by the suggestion that it may tap into the same selective attention or cognitive control mechanisms that yield congruency effects in Stroop and Flanker paradigms. Here, we report data from 130 subjects on the VHPT-F, several versions of Stroop and Flanker tasks, as well as fluid IQ. Results suggested a small degree of shared variance in Stroop and Flanker congruency effects, which did not relate to congruency effects on the VHPT-F. Variability on the VHPT-F was also not correlated with Fluid IQ. In sum, we find no evidence that holistic face processing as measured by congruency in the VHPT-F is accounted for by domain-general control mechanisms. 相似文献
72.
Lean back and relax: Reclined seating position buffers the effect of frustration on anger and aggression 下载免费PDF全文
Frustration is a powerful instigator of anger‐based aggression. We hypothesized that the impact of a frustration on anger and aggressive behavior is reduced in a state of feeling relaxed, which is considered incompatible with the experience of anger. Seventy‐nine participants received frustrating feedback either when sitting upright or sitting in a reclined position and were then given a chance to act aggressively toward the frustrator. Feelings of anger and relaxation were assessed before and after the frustration. Participants in the reclined position felt more relaxed than those sitting upright, which indirectly predicted less aggressive behavior via lower anger. The results are consistent with theories of incompatible states and embodiment and have implications for using body‐related cues to mitigate anger‐based aggression. 相似文献
73.
74.
Maria Jesús Blasco Gemma Vilagut Jos Almenara Miquel Roca Jos Antonio Piqueras Andrea Gabilondo Carolina Lagares Victoria Soto‐Sanz Itxaso Alayo Carlos G. Forero Enrique Echeburúa Margalida Gili Ana Isabel Cebri Ronny Bruffaerts Randy P. Auerbach Matthew K. Nock Ronald C. Kessler Jordi Alonso 《Suicide & life-threatening behavior》2019,49(3):881-898
75.
In a mental rotation task, participants must determine whether two stimuli match when one undergoes a rotation in 3-D space
relative to the other. The key evidence for mental rotation is the finding of a linear increase in response times as objects
are rotated farther apart. This signature increase in response times is also found in recognition of rotated objects, which
has led many theorists to postulate mental rotation as a key transformational procedure in object recognition. We compared
mental rotation and object recognition in tasks that used the same stimuli and presentation conditions and found that, whereas
mental rotation costs increased relatively linearly with rotation, object recognition costs increased only over small rotations.
Taken in conjunction with a recent brain imaging study, this dissociation in behavioral performance suggests that object recognition
is based on matching of image features rather than on 3-D mental transformations. 相似文献
76.
Font tuning (FT) occurs when observers recognize a sequence of letters presented in the same font faster than in different fonts (Sanocki 1987, 1988 Journal of Experimental Psychology. Human Perception and Performance 13 267-278; 14 472-480). Here, we test the hypothesis that FT is associated with expertise with a specific writing system. We developed a systematic search task allowing the measurement of FT over a large number of letters and generalized the finding of FT from English readers viewing Roman letters to Chinese readers viewing Chinese characters. Non-Chinese readers did not show evidence of FT for Chinese characters in this search task. We also used a simpler 3-letter identification task to directly compare novice and expert readers, and to explore FT for different aspects of font such as fill, slant, and aspect ratio. We found that experts tune to aspect ratio but not to the other font changes. These findings reveal that letters are not processed visually in the same manner as shapes, and suggest an explanation for the cortical specialization obtained in the visual system for letters. 相似文献
77.
Research on the belief in a just world (BJW) has focused on its intra-individual functions (e.g., psychological well-being) and its inter-individual consequences (e.g., derogation of victims). Recent theorizing, however, has indicated that the BJW may also have more societal functions and consequences, serving as a legitimizing device of the status quo. The studies in the current paper focus on this latter view and are based on Alves and Correia's ( 2008 ) research which found that the expression of high personal and general belief in a just world is injunctively normative. Two experimental studies aimed at ascertaining three issues: (1) the dimension(s) of social value (social utility and/or social desirability) on which the BJW normativity anchors; (2) whether the expression of moderate BJW is also injunctively normative; and (3) whether the injunctive normativity of the BJW is related to perceptions of truth. Results indicate that moderate and high personal and general BJW are normative. Yet, whereas the normativity of personal BJW anchors both on social utility and social desirability, that of general BJW anchors only on social utility. We discuss personal and general BJW as judgement norms, whose normativity may not be personally acknowledged (in the case of general BJW) and does not necessarily derive from being perceived as true, but from the fact that such norms carry social value at least in individualistic societies. 相似文献
78.
79.
Alain Gauthier 《World Futures: Journal of General Evolution》2013,69(1):20-28
The purpose here is to explore why and how to develop an emerging form of integral leadership—called evolutionary co-leadership—that is responsibly dedicated to the co-evolution of life and humanity on Planet Earth. After offering some fresh distinctions about leadership, this article calls for a shift to a radically new development paradigm, and identifies key qualities and practices of evolutionary co-leadership. It then shares some views on the multiplying effects of an integral approach to co-leadership development—in the context of transforming education—and concludes with some avenues to disseminate this emerging leadership form, mainly to deal constructively with what Barbara Marx Hubbard (2001) calls the current “birthing pains” of a new civilization. 相似文献
80.
Clara Rocha Luis C. Dias Isabel Dimas 《Journal of Multi-Criteria Decision Analysis》2013,20(1-2):13-27
This work proposes an approach to cluster and sort a set of alternatives considering multi‐criteria categories with a partial order structure. It can be considered a heuristic approach because it does not attempt to derive an optimal partial order among all conceivable clusters of alternatives. Rather than this, it intends to be a simple approach that is transparent to the Decision Maker (DM) whose assistance is sought to help shaping the results. The approach proposed arises from the conjugation of traditional Clustering analysis and Multi‐criteria sorting tools. At the outset, the number of categories and their characteristics is unknown. First, we need to detect only the clusters themselves on the basis of a similarity measure independent of the preferences of the DM. Next, we detect potential partial order relations that might exist between them, according to the subjective preferences of the DM. Such preferences are elicited only after the DM has examined the clusters detected and deemed that these categories made sense. The new approach performs very well in a real‐world problem of management of intragroup conflicts and conflict handling strategies. Copyright © 2012 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd. 相似文献