首页 | 本学科首页   官方微博 | 高级检索  
文章检索
  按 检索   检索词:      
出版年份:   被引次数:   他引次数: 提示:输入*表示无穷大
  收费全文   496篇
  免费   16篇
  国内免费   1篇
  2018年   5篇
  2017年   7篇
  2016年   7篇
  2015年   6篇
  2014年   5篇
  2013年   54篇
  2012年   6篇
  2011年   14篇
  2010年   7篇
  2009年   5篇
  2008年   9篇
  2007年   19篇
  2006年   16篇
  2005年   13篇
  2004年   15篇
  2003年   12篇
  2002年   13篇
  2001年   19篇
  2000年   14篇
  1999年   11篇
  1998年   10篇
  1997年   6篇
  1996年   11篇
  1995年   9篇
  1994年   8篇
  1993年   7篇
  1992年   11篇
  1991年   14篇
  1990年   8篇
  1988年   7篇
  1987年   7篇
  1986年   6篇
  1985年   9篇
  1984年   11篇
  1983年   5篇
  1981年   7篇
  1980年   4篇
  1979年   14篇
  1978年   3篇
  1977年   6篇
  1976年   4篇
  1975年   7篇
  1973年   18篇
  1972年   6篇
  1971年   12篇
  1970年   3篇
  1969年   5篇
  1968年   8篇
  1967年   6篇
  1966年   7篇
排序方式: 共有513条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
71.
Auditory temporal psychometric functions were obtained for forward masking, backward masking, and for combined forward and backward masking for the case of an impulsive signal in white noise. Temporal psychometric functions show a large interaction between forward masking and backward masking when evaluated against an independence model of performance. The results are not consistent with the concept of a sharp rectangular temporal integrating period (“moment”) to account for the interaction.  相似文献   
72.
73.
The depth of visual information processing is identified with the longest randomly-generated binary-encoded spatial pattern whose partial repetitions can be detected. In contrast with the auditory detection of sequentially-presented constraints, the depth of perceptible visual encoding of spatially-presented constraints is sharply limited. Depths of about 35 were achieved for one-dimensional constraints; depths of perhaps 3 or 4 were achieved for two-dimensional constraints imposed in only one direction and depths of perhaps 2 for two-dimensional constraints imposed in two directions. With one-dimensional constraints, it is shown that the inferred depth of processing is partially determined by the number of pattern representations and by the spatial distance between successive representation for visual displays of fixed size.  相似文献   
74.
High-frequency seven-letter words were displayed letter by letter in scrambled temporal orders upon an oscilloscope. After each incorrect identification, the sequence was repeated. Highest word-identification scores were obtained with initial and terminal starting positions, with shortest and longest spacingbetween successive letters, and at the slowest and fastest rates of letter presentation. The relative contribution of each variable was maintained in combination with other variables. The conditional probability of a correct word identification, given that the word was not identified on a previous presentation, plunges sharply with successive presentations. This apparent nonindependence of performance over successive presentations is, however, shown to be an artifact of pooling the results of heterogeneous experimental variables.  相似文献   
75.
76.
The loudness of a 1000-Hz tone at weak sound pressures and its softness at intense ones were scaled by the method of magnitude estimation. Estimates of loudness plotted as a function of sound pressure on double logarithmic coordinates showed the well-documented steepening near threshold. A similar steepening occurred in the softness function at high intensities. Stevens’ law, which is linear on these coordinates, could be recovered either by translating the pressure scale to a new origin, or by translating the response scale. Translations of the response scale were preferred because they produced functions in which loudness and softness were reciprocally related, whereas transformations of the stimulus made the softness function almost three times as steep as the loudness function.  相似文献   
77.
Ss were presented sets of numbers said to represent sample price information from grocery stores. Ss rated the relative desirability of shopping at each of a series of stores and were also asked to make paired-comparison preference choices. In Experiment I, some sets contained favorable information, some contained unfavorable information, and some contained neutral information added to the favorable or unfavorable information. The addition of neutral information led to a decrease in the polarity of responses. This is consistent with an averaging, but not an adding, formulation of how the information is integrated. In Experiment II, sets of favorable or unfavorable information varied in size and a set-size effect was obtained. The greater the amount of favorable or unfavorable information, the more extreme the response. This was accounted for by assuming that an initial neutral expectancy is averaged with the information presented.  相似文献   
78.
79.
Hodgkinson (1998) argues that we should have transformed the SINDSCAL source weights we used for an analysis presented on page 15 of our paper on the judgement of HIV cases (Irwin, Jones, and Mundo, 1996). In this reply we present theoretical and empirical arguments for why we did not transform the weights. Our statements are presented in the context of a more general discussion of how source weights should be interpreted and used. © 1998 John Wiley & Sons, Ltd.  相似文献   
80.
设为首页 | 免责声明 | 关于勤云 | 加入收藏

Copyright©北京勤云科技发展有限公司  京ICP备09084417号