全文获取类型
收费全文 | 589篇 |
免费 | 26篇 |
专业分类
615篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 6篇 |
2020年 | 13篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 22篇 |
2017年 | 19篇 |
2016年 | 15篇 |
2015年 | 13篇 |
2014年 | 13篇 |
2013年 | 90篇 |
2012年 | 27篇 |
2011年 | 31篇 |
2010年 | 23篇 |
2009年 | 17篇 |
2008年 | 33篇 |
2007年 | 18篇 |
2006年 | 20篇 |
2005年 | 23篇 |
2004年 | 17篇 |
2003年 | 20篇 |
2002年 | 18篇 |
2001年 | 4篇 |
2000年 | 10篇 |
1999年 | 6篇 |
1998年 | 9篇 |
1997年 | 8篇 |
1996年 | 8篇 |
1995年 | 4篇 |
1994年 | 6篇 |
1993年 | 4篇 |
1992年 | 7篇 |
1991年 | 10篇 |
1990年 | 7篇 |
1988年 | 4篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1983年 | 6篇 |
1982年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 5篇 |
1980年 | 4篇 |
1978年 | 4篇 |
1977年 | 3篇 |
1975年 | 4篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1971年 | 4篇 |
1969年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1963年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有615条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
81.
A Test of Order-Constrained Hypotheses for Circular Data With Applications to Human Movement Science
Researchers studying the movements of the human body often encounter data measured in angles (e.g., angular displacements of joints). The evaluation of these circular data requires special statistical methods. The authors introduce a new test for the analysis of order-constrained hypotheses for circular data. Through this test, researchers can evaluate their expectations regarding the outcome of an experiment directly by representing their ideas in the form of a hypothesis containing inequality constraints. The resulting data analysis is generally more powerful than one using standard null hypothesis testing. Two examples of circular data from human movement science are presented to illustrate the use of the test. Results from a simulation study show that the test performs well. 相似文献
82.
Roberta L. Klatzky Brian McCloskey Sally Doherty James Pellegrino Terence Smith 《Journal of motor behavior》2013,45(2):187-213
Our two experiments investigated associations between cognitive representations of objects and hand-shape categories. Hand configurations were partitioned according to prehensility and the size of the contacting surface, resulting in the classes: pinch, poke, palm, and clench. Experiment 1 elicited object names in response to configuration-name cues, provided ratings of the relevance of each configuration to a set of objects, and probed for the functions determining such relevance. Cueing with a configuration class elicited an associated object category with substantial intersubject agreement, and vice versa. Both the object categories and the functions associated with the four hand-configuration classes differed substantially, although the same object could be associated to some extent with multiple configurations, given variations in function. Experiment 2 elicited the names of hand-configuration classes in response to unfamiliar forms, which varied systematically in depth and the size of the projecting picture-plane surface. The modal response, response time, and degree of intersubject agreement were directly related to these variables. These structural variables, however, did not adequately predict shaping responses to real objects, as ascertained from Experiment 1. The results have implications for cognitive representation of motor categories and hand shaping in response to objects. 相似文献
83.
The present communication is made in response to a published study (Lester and Schumacher, 1969) which found no support for hypotheses that schizophrenic women might usefully be conceptualized as dying women, and that schizophrenics (men and women) would have more death themes in their TAT stories than non-schizophrenics. The present authors argue that there is no reason to expect schizophrenic women to feel like dying women, do present empirical data strongly supporting the notion of a relation between feelings of unreality and of deadness, and suggest reasons for Lester and Schumacher's failure to find schizophrenic-nonschizophrenic differences in TAT themes of death. 相似文献
84.
Miss Irene Sinanoglou 《Psychoanalytic Psychotherapy》2013,27(1):27-37
SUMMARY Institutions can be used by their individual members to strengthen mechanisms of defence, especially against early paranoid and depressive anxieties. This is not to imply that institutios become psychotic, but that we would expect to find in them manifestations of unreality, splitting, hostility, suspicion and other forms of maladaptive behaviour which are the equivalent of psychotic symptoms of an individual. This paper describes some of the attitudes of those who care for psychotic patients in a hospital setting. These attitudes seem to be influenced by basic fears that are provoked by the relationship with the patients and by coming in contact with their needs and disturbance from a position of responsibility. Such fears are accompanied by phantasies and are related to specific defence mechanisms. 相似文献
85.
Sergio Morra Elisa Bisagno Sara Caviola Chiara Delfante Irene Cristina Mammarella 《认知与教导》2013,31(4):483-511
AbstractThis article reconsiders Case’s theory of central conceptual structures (CCS), examining the relation between working memory and the acquisition of quantitative CCS. The lead hypothesis is that the development of working memory capacity shapes the development of quantitative concepts (whole and rational numbers). Study I, with 779 children from preschool to grade 5, validated a measure of the whole number CCS and found that children’s understanding of whole number advances by one developmental level per additional unit of working memory capacity. Study II, with 92 sixth- and seventh-graders, found that a test of the rational number CCS was predicted by working memory capacity and, to a lesser extent, by intrusion errors in the listening span. We also identified 2 subsets of items that demand a capacity of 4 or 5 units, respectively. Overall, the results support Case’s CCS theory and clarify the role of working memory in the acquisition of numerical concepts. The relevance of these results in relation to the current debate is discussed, with extensive connections to other current theories of whole or rational number comprehension. 相似文献
86.
Memory was investigated in 27 children and adolescents in a longitudinal study before and at one and two years after epilepsy surgery. A comparison group with intractable epilepsy (n = 15), matched in age, sex, age of seizure onset, and IQ, was studied at comparable points in time. Methods included administration of standardized measures of story recall and face recognition, and qualitative interviews to probe the participants’ perceptions of their memory function over time. There was no significant change over time in either group on the standardized measures. Site and laterality of excision, age at surgery, and seizure outcome were not predictive of change within the surgical group. The narratives revealed facets of memory that were problematic in daily life. The objective and subjective results showed low concordance regarding change, possibly due to limitations in the objective measures for capturing semantic and autobiographical memory. 相似文献
87.
Mirko Pesce Lorenza Speranza Sara Franceschelli Valentina Ialenti Irene Iezzi Antonia Patruno Alessia Rizzuto Claudio Robazza Maria Anna De Lutiis Mario Felaco Alfredo Grilli 《Aggressive behavior》2013,39(2):141-148
Recently, several studies reported a relationship between immune system activation and anger expression. Consequently, the aim of this study was to explore immunitary molecular mechanisms that potentially underlie anger expression. To this end, we applied the Frustration—Aggression Theory in a contact sport model, utilizing the nearing of sporting events to trigger anger feelings. In parallel, we evaluated the activation of immune system at mRNA levels. We enrolled 20 amateur rugby players (age ± SD, 27.2 ± 4.5) who underwent psychological assessment to evaluate anger, with the State‐Trait Anger Expression Inventory‐2 (STAXI‐2), before rugby matches; at the same time blood samples were taken to analyze the variations of gene expression by microarray. During the 2 hr before each game, a significant increase was verified in the Rage State (RS) score compared to the score ascertained 72 hr before. At the same time, we found modulation in expression profile, in particular increased expression of gene that encodes interleukin l‐β (IL‐1β). In a regression analysis, RS score was related to IL‐1β, and the potential risk factors age, body mass index, smoking, and drinking. The levels of cytokine were positively and independently related to RS score. Our results suggest that the nearing of sporting event can trigger anger state feelings and activate immune system in rugby players. We propose the IL‐1β as a potential biological marker of anger. However, further research is necessary to clarify the correlation between cytokine and anger. Aggr. Behav. 39 :141‐148, 2013. © 2012 Wiley Periodicals, Inc. 相似文献
88.
89.
Visuospatial working memory (VSWM) and visual perception were examined in two groups aged 11–13, one with children displaying symptoms of nonverbal learning disability (NLD) (n = 18) and the other a control group without learning disabilities (n = 18). The two groups were matched for general verbal abilities, age, gender, and socioeconomic level. The children were presented with VSWM tests involving visual and spatial-simultaneous processes, and also with a classical visual illusion, a classical ambiguous figure, as well as visual perception tests specifically devised for the present study. Results revealed that performance of children at risk of NLD was worse than controls in some VSWM and in visual perception tests without memory involvement; these latter required comparisons of visual stimuli and locations in space with distractors. Moreover, the two groups differed in perceiving the classical ambiguous figure. Findings are discussed in the light of both theoretical and clinical implications. 相似文献
90.
Abstract Two experiments examined the learning of a set of Greek pronunciation rules through explicit and implicit modes of rule presentation. Experiment 1 compared the effectiveness of implicit and explicit modes of presentation in two modalities, visual and auditory. Subjects in the explicit or rule group were presented with the rule set, and those in the implicit or natural group were shown a set of Greek words, composed of letters from the rule set, linked to their pronunciations. Subjects learned the Greek words to criterion and were then given a series of tests which aimed to tap different types of knowledge. The results showed an advantage of explicit study of the rules. In addition, an interaction was found between mode of presentation and modality. Explicit instruction was more effective in the visual than in the auditory modality, whereas there was no modality effect for implicit instruction. Experiment 2 examined a possible reason for the advantage of the rule groups by comparing different combinations of explicit and implicit presentation in the study and learning phases. The results suggested that explicit presentation of the rules is only beneficial when it is followed by practice at applying them. 相似文献