全文获取类型
收费全文 | 158篇 |
免费 | 9篇 |
专业分类
167篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 3篇 |
2022年 | 1篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 9篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 14篇 |
2017年 | 8篇 |
2016年 | 5篇 |
2015年 | 4篇 |
2014年 | 6篇 |
2013年 | 17篇 |
2012年 | 6篇 |
2011年 | 11篇 |
2010年 | 6篇 |
2009年 | 9篇 |
2008年 | 7篇 |
2007年 | 11篇 |
2006年 | 5篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 4篇 |
2003年 | 1篇 |
2002年 | 3篇 |
2001年 | 1篇 |
2000年 | 1篇 |
1999年 | 1篇 |
1998年 | 2篇 |
1997年 | 2篇 |
1996年 | 1篇 |
1995年 | 1篇 |
1994年 | 1篇 |
1993年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 1篇 |
1989年 | 1篇 |
1986年 | 1篇 |
1984年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 1篇 |
排序方式: 共有167条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
91.
Patricia Novo Navarro Alejandro Maiche Marini Jan Scott Ramón Landin-Romero Benedikt L. Amann 《Personality and individual differences》2013
Horizontal eye movement is an essential component of the psychological intervention “eye movement desensitization and reprocessing” (EMDR) used in posttraumatic stress disorder. A hypothesized mechanism of action is an overload of the visuospatial sketchpad and/or the phonological loop of the working memory. 相似文献
92.
Rene A. Middleton Holly A. Stadler Carol Simpson Yuh‐Jen Guo Michele J. Brown Germayne Crow Kelly Schuck Yared Alemu Alejandro A. Lazarte 《Journal of counseling and development : JCD》2005,83(4):444-456
This study documents the relationship between White racial identity development and multicultural counseling competency (MCC) as reported by mental health practitioners. Initial results were generally consistent with J. E. Helms's (1990) construction of White racial identity attitude development theory. More sophisticated statuses of White racial identity development generally correlated with higher levels of perceived MCC. Overall, there was a significant difference in MCC reported between men and women. Among counselors, conflicting relationships were observed between some racial identity statuses and multicultural competencies (skills, relationship). 相似文献
93.
Ernesto San Martín Alejandro Jara Jean-Marie Rolin Michel Mouchart 《Psychometrika》2011,76(3):385-409
We study the identification and consistency of Bayesian semiparametric IRT-type models, where the uncertainty on the abilities’
distribution is modeled using a prior distribution on the space of probability measures. We show that for the semiparametric
Rasch Poisson counts model, simple restrictions ensure the identification of a general distribution generating the abilities,
even for a finite number of probes. For the semiparametric Rasch model, only a finite number of properties of the general
abilities’ distribution can be identified by a finite number of items, which are completely characterized. The full identification
of the semiparametric Rasch model can be only achieved when an infinite number of items is available. The results are illustrated
using simulated data. 相似文献
94.
95.
Troncoso J Múnera A Delgado-García JM 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2004,11(6):724-726
The murine vibrissae sensorimotor system has been scrutinized as a target of motor learning through trace classical conditioning. Conditioned eyelid responses were acquired by using weak electrical whisker-pad stimulation as conditioned stimulus (CS) and strong electrical periorbital stimulation as unconditioned stimulus (US). In addition, conditioned vibrissal protraction was obtained pairing either weak electrical whisker-pad stimulation or a tone as CS, with a strong electric shock delivered in the whisker-pad as US. This finding suggests that evolutionary pressure has selected a sensorimotor system capable of constructing conditioned responses on the basis of temporal relationships of stimuli, independently of any putative functional purpose. 相似文献
96.
Habit and skill learning in schizophrenia: evidence of normal striatal processing with abnormal cortical input 总被引:3,自引:0,他引:3 下载免费PDF全文
Weickert TW Terrazas A Bigelow LB Malley JD Hyde T Egan MF Weinberger DR Goldberg TE 《Learning & memory (Cold Spring Harbor, N.Y.)》2002,9(6):430-442
Different forms of nondeclarative learning involve regionally specific striatal circuits. The motor circuit (involving the putamen) has been associated with motor–skill learning and the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (DLPFC) circuit (involving the caudate) has been associated with cognitive–habit learning. Efforts to differentiate functional striatal circuits within patient samples have been limited. Previous studies have provided mixed results regarding striatal-dependent nondeclarative learning deficits in patients with schizophrenia. In this study, a cognitive–habit learning task (probabilistic weather prediction) was used to assess the DLPFC circuit and a motor–skill learning task (pursuit rotor) was used to assess the motor circuit in 35 patients with schizophrenia and 35 normal controls. Patients with schizophrenia displayed significant performance differences from controls on both nondeclarative tasks; however, cognitive–habit learning rate in patients did not differ from controls. There were performance and learning-rate differences on the motor–skill learning task between the whole sample of patients and controls, however, analysis of a subset of patients and controls matched on general intellectual level eliminated learning rate differences between groups. The abnormal performance offset between patients with schizophrenia and controls in the absence of learning rate differences suggests that abnormal cortical processing provides altered input to normal striatal circuitry. 相似文献
97.
The Simon effect (SE) usually is described as the performance advantage that results when a target and its associated response share the same spatial code, as opposed to when they do not, even when the target's spatial code is task-irrelevant. To some, this task-irrelevant code represents the location of the target with respect to the participant (Simon & Craft, 1970), whereas to others (Umiltà & Nicoletti, 1992) it represents the location of the target with respect to the locus of attention. By using a spatial cuing procedure, we simultaneously tested both of these hypotheses and found evidence that both types of codes produce independent SEs, therefore providing evidence that multiple spatial codes can simultaneously influence performance in a Simon task. 相似文献
98.
Ivar R. Hannikainen Kevin P. Tobia Guilherme da F. C. F. de Almeida Raff Donelson Vilius Dranseika Markus Kneer Niek Strohmaier Piotr Bystranowski Kristina Dolinina Bartosz Janik Sothie Keo Eglė Lauraitytė Alice Liefgreen Maciej Próchnicki Alejandro Rosas Noel Struchiner 《Cognitive Science》2021,45(8):e13024
Despite pervasive variation in the content of laws, legal theorists and anthropologists have argued that laws share certain abstract features and even speculated that law may be a human universal. In the present report, we evaluate this thesis through an experiment administered in 11 different countries. Are there cross-cultural principles of law? In a between-subjects design, participants (N = 3,054) were asked whether there could be laws that violate certain procedural principles (e.g., laws applied retrospectively or unintelligible laws), and also whether there are any such laws. Confirming our preregistered prediction, people reported that such laws cannot exist, but also (paradoxically) that there are such laws. These results document cross-culturally and –linguistically robust beliefs about the concept of law which defy people's grasp of how legal systems function in practice. 相似文献
99.
José Antonio Cecchini Alejandro Carriedo Javier Fernández-Río Antonio Méndez-Giménez Carmen González Beatriz Sánchez-Martínez Pablo Rodríguez-González 《International Journal of Clinical and Health Psychology》2021,21(1):100200
Background/Objective: The aim was to evaluate the evolution of depressive symptoms and to explore the influence of physical activity (PA) practice during the home confinement due to the COVID-19 outbreak in Spain. Method: Information was collected from 595 confined participants who reported personal and contextual information, depressive symptoms and PA levels at four time points. Results: The changes in depressive symptoms were analyzed using a linear mixed model with cubic splines. Results showed a significant increase, with a significant rise between T1 and T2 (OR = 2.38, 95% CI = 1.83-3.10). It continued growing until T4 (OR = 2.93, 95% CI = 1.97-4.38). A negative relationship was observed between the increase in depressive symptoms and moderate-to-vigorous physical activity (MVPA) levels, with a significant slope up to 4 hours of MVPA per week (OR = 0.51, 95% CI = 0.29-0.90) that tended to increase until 16 hours per week of MVPA (OR = 0.41, 95% CI = 0.20-0.87). Conclusions: Results from a partition model showed that moderate intensity of PA could be enough to prevent an increase of depressive symptoms during home isolation. 相似文献
100.
Gavin Jun Peng Ng Alejandro Lleras Simona Buetti 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》2018,80(7):1752-1762
Stage 1 processing in visual search (e.g., efficient search) has long been thought to be unaffected by factors such as set size or lure–distractor similarity (or at least to be only minimally affected). Recent research from Buetti, Cronin, Madison, Wang, and Lleras (Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 145, 672–707, 2016) showed that in efficient visual search with a fixed target, reaction times increase logarithmically as a function of set size and, further, that the slope of these logarithmic functions is modulated by target–distractor similarity. This has led to the proposal that the cognitive architecture of Stage 1 processing is parallel, of unlimited capacity, and exhaustive in nature. Such an architecture produces reaction time functions that increase logarithmically with set size (as opposed to being unaffected by it). However, in the previous studies, eye movements were not monitored. It is thus possible that the logarithmicity of the reaction time functions emerged simply as an artifact of eye movements rather than as a reflection of the underlying cognitive architecture. Here we ruled out the possibility that eye movements resulted in the observed logarithmic functions, by asking participants to keep their eyes at fixation while completing fixed-target efficient visual search tasks. The logarithmic RT functions still emerged even when participants were not allowed to make eye movements, thus providing further support for our proposal. Additionally, we found that search efficiency is slightly improved when eye movements are restricted and lure–target similarity is relatively high. 相似文献