全文获取类型
收费全文 | 319篇 |
免费 | 1篇 |
专业分类
320篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 4篇 |
2023年 | 3篇 |
2019年 | 5篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 7篇 |
2016年 | 12篇 |
2015年 | 8篇 |
2014年 | 11篇 |
2013年 | 38篇 |
2012年 | 10篇 |
2011年 | 14篇 |
2010年 | 10篇 |
2009年 | 6篇 |
2008年 | 13篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 13篇 |
2004年 | 14篇 |
2003年 | 15篇 |
2002年 | 6篇 |
2001年 | 14篇 |
2000年 | 8篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 3篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 1篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1990年 | 3篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1987年 | 3篇 |
1986年 | 2篇 |
1985年 | 1篇 |
1983年 | 3篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1981年 | 1篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1979年 | 3篇 |
1977年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1975年 | 3篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1972年 | 1篇 |
1971年 | 3篇 |
1970年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 1篇 |
1966年 | 1篇 |
1965年 | 2篇 |
排序方式: 共有320条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
141.
Inge Houkes Yvonne H. W. M. Winants Mascha Twellaar 《Journal of Occupational & Organizational Psychology》2008,81(2):249-276
This study aimed to develop and test a specific pattern of relationships between job demands, job resources and person‐related factors on the one hand, and the three burnout dimensions on the other, among Dutch General Practitioners. In addition, we aimed to test whether gender differences exist in this regard. Based on several theoretical models such as the Job Demand‐Control model and the Job Demands‐Resources model of burnout as well as a review of burnout studies among physicians, we formulated a research model of burnout. The research questions were answered by means of self‐report questionnaires using a full panel design with two waves. Cross‐lagged panel analyses indicated that the causal direction of the relationships between demands, resources and person‐related factors on the one hand, and burnout on the other is reciprocal. In addition, multi‐sample analyses revealed that the pattern of relationships between job demands, job resources, person‐related factors and burnout is different for men and women, although results are less clear at the second measurement point. Among other things, we recommend anticipating in a gender sensitive way on risk factors for burnout and motivation loss for young professionals by coaching and empowerment in vocational training. 相似文献
142.
Psychodynamic group therapy offers many opportunities for members to identify, understand, and change the underlying problems,
which lead to their aggressive behavior. In addition, group therapy can help to improve the members’ interpersonal relationships.
The “Coping with Aggression” is comprised of three groups. A psychodynamic group of assaultive men made up the first group.
The second group consisted of two leaders and a consultant whom the group members knew about but never actually met. The observers
who sat outside the group made up the third group. Therapy contracts, boundaries, and leadership, and group process are discussed. 相似文献
143.
Farnaz Kaighobadi Todd K. Shackelford Danielle Popp Ryan M. Moyer Vincent M. Bates James R. Liddle 《Journal of research in personality》2009,43(6):1033-1039
We hypothesized that men’s personality traits interact with men’s perceived risk of partner’s infidelity to predict men’s partner-directed violence. Moderation analyses of data provided by 467 men in a committed relationship indicated that: (1) men with lower emotional stability, agreeableness, and conscientiousness, and men who perceived greater risk of partner infidelity perpetrated more partner-directed violence, and (2) the relationship between men’s personality traits and partner-directed violence depends on their perceptions of the risk of partner infidelity. Simple slope analyses indicated that: (a) men’s emotional stability and agreeableness predict partner-directed violence only when perceived risk of partner infidelity is low, and (b) men’s conscientiousness predicts partner-directed violence only when perceived risk of partner infidelity is high. 相似文献
144.
Pettit GS Erath SA Lansford JE Dodge KA Bates JE 《International journal of behavioral development》2011,35(6):482-489
The predictive relations between social capital depth (high-quality relationships across contexts) and breadth (friendship network extensivity) and early-adult, life adjustment outcomes were examined using data from a prospective longitudinal study. Interviews at age 22 yielded (a) psychometrically sound indexes of relationship quality with parents, peers, and romantic partners that served as indicators of a latent construct of social capital depth, and (b) a measure of number of close friends. In follow-up interviews at age 24, participants reported on their behavioral adjustment, educational attainment, and arrests and illicit substance use. Early-adolescent assessments of behavioral adjustment and academic performance served as controls; data on what were construed as interpersonal assets (teacher-rated social skills) and opportunities (family income) were also collected at this time. Results showed that depth was associated with overall better young-adult adjustment, net of prior adjustment, and assets and opportunities. Breadth was only modestly associated with later outcomes, and when its overlap with depth was taken into account, breadth predicted higher levels of subsequent externalizing problems. These findings are consistent with the notion that social capital is multidimensional and that elements of it confer distinct benefits during an important life transition. 相似文献
145.
Egeland J Landrø NI Sundet K Asbjørnsen A Lund A Roness A Rund BR 《Scandinavian journal of psychology》2005,46(2):201-208
Studies of localized brain dysfunctions have revealed connections between patterns of cognitive dysfunction and specific profiles of memory impairment. The amnesic type of memory impairment is defined by deficits in both free recall and recognition memory, whereas the dysexecutive memory impairment is characterized by retrieval deficits, i.e. a disproportional impairment in free recall relative to recognition memory. The present study tests whether classifications of psychiatric patients into recall impaired only (= RO group) and Recall and Recognition impaired (= RR group) correspond to the executive type and amnesic type of memory impairment. The alternative hypothesis is that the two groups merely differ in degree of neuropsychological and psychiatric disturbance. Forty-four subjects impaired on California Verbal Learning Test (CVLT) were selected from a larger database of 103 impaired and non-impaired subjects with schizophrenia or recurrent major depression. Subjects were classified into RO and RR groups and compared on measures of memory strategy (recency effect and interference on CVLT), overall neuropsychological function (Stroop Test and WAIS-R similarity) and psychiatric symptom load (positive and negative symptoms). Repeated measures ANOVA showed no effects of group, i.e. the RR group did not perform consistently below the RO group with regard to memory strategy, neuropsychological function or psychiatric symptom load. Two out of three analyses showed group x test interaction, supporting the dissociation of distinct executive and amnesic profiles among psychiatric patients. The RO group was more susceptible to interference but had better recency score than the RR group. The RO had higher negative symptoms while the RR group had higher positive symptoms. 相似文献
146.
The Servant Leadership Survey: Development and Validation of a Multidimensional Measure 总被引:2,自引:0,他引:2
Purpose
The purpose of this paper is to describe the development and validation of a multi-dimensional instrument to measure servant leadership. 相似文献147.
The cognitive consequences of forming implementation intentions in controlling fear were addressed in the present study. Participants with an intense fear of spiders evaluated pictures of spiders, pleasant pictures, and neutral pictures under cognitive load. Regulatory control was measured by participants' self-report ratings of the pictures on the Self-Assessment Manikins Scales. Only participants given implementation intentions reported weaker negative emotional responses to the pictures of spiders as compared to participants given a goal intention and to no-goal control participants. Thus, emotional control by implementation intentions was shown not to tax a person's cognitive resources, attesting to the automatic nature of this self-regulation strategy. 相似文献
148.
Work during the past decade has suggested an association between panic disorder and suicide (i.e., suicidal ideation and suicide attempts) that cannot simply be accounted for by co-occurring depression symptoms. To clarify the linkage between panic disorder and suicide, the association between panic-specific clinical and cognitive variables and suicide indicators were evaluated in patients with panic disorder (N=146). Analyses predicting the presence of suicidal ideation (positive, negative) after covarying the effects of a current mood disorder diagnosis and depression symptoms indicated a number of significant predictors including: (1) overall anxiety symptoms; (2) level of anticipatory anxiety; (3) avoidance of bodily sensations; (4) attentional vigilance toward bodily perturbations; and (5) phrenophobia (i.e., fear of cognitive incapacitation). Anxiety-specific variables did not account for unique variance in predicting prior history of suicide attempts. 相似文献
149.
150.
Response set membership contributes much to the interference in the color-word Stroop task. This may be due to selective allocation
of attention to eligible responses or, alternatively, to greater inhibition of distractors that are not responses. In the
present article, we report two experiments that were designed to adjudicate between these accounts. In Experiment 1, membership
was manipulated on a trial-by-trial basis by cuing the possible responses for each trial. Response time (RT) was longer for
distractors that corresponded to a cued, eligible response than to an ineligible one. This cuing effect was independent of
the number of different responses. In Experiment 2, the distractor was cued on half the trials. Cuing the distractor decreased
RTs on both incongruent and congruent trials. Vincentile analyses in both experiments revealed that the effects were constant
throughout the entire RT distributions. These results suggest that response set effects arise because of selective allocation
of attention to eligible responses. 相似文献