全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2227篇 |
免费 | 28篇 |
专业分类
2255篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 14篇 |
2023年 | 11篇 |
2022年 | 13篇 |
2021年 | 16篇 |
2020年 | 49篇 |
2019年 | 63篇 |
2018年 | 68篇 |
2017年 | 73篇 |
2016年 | 76篇 |
2015年 | 53篇 |
2014年 | 68篇 |
2013年 | 254篇 |
2012年 | 97篇 |
2011年 | 103篇 |
2010年 | 69篇 |
2009年 | 82篇 |
2008年 | 90篇 |
2007年 | 94篇 |
2006年 | 64篇 |
2005年 | 52篇 |
2004年 | 64篇 |
2003年 | 53篇 |
2002年 | 61篇 |
2001年 | 41篇 |
2000年 | 37篇 |
1999年 | 33篇 |
1998年 | 29篇 |
1997年 | 44篇 |
1996年 | 32篇 |
1995年 | 18篇 |
1994年 | 21篇 |
1993年 | 18篇 |
1992年 | 29篇 |
1991年 | 31篇 |
1990年 | 18篇 |
1989年 | 15篇 |
1988年 | 19篇 |
1986年 | 13篇 |
1985年 | 26篇 |
1984年 | 22篇 |
1983年 | 21篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 13篇 |
1979年 | 12篇 |
1978年 | 20篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 11篇 |
1975年 | 10篇 |
1974年 | 23篇 |
1973年 | 11篇 |
排序方式: 共有2255条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
101.
Ian O’Flynn 《Res Publica》2010,16(3):299-315
Although the idea of the public interest features prominently in many accounts of deliberative democracy, the relationship
between deliberative democracy and the public interest is rarely spelt out with any degree of precision. In this article,
I identify and defend one particular way of framing this relationship. I begin by arguing that people can deliberate about
the public interest only if the public interest is, in principle, identifiable independently of their deliberations. Of course,
some pluralists claim that the public interest is an implausible idea, which casts doubt on the idea that there might be something
for people to deliberate about. Yet while, following Brian Barry, we can get around this problem by defining the public interest
as an interest in which everyone shares qua member of the public, what still needs to be explained is why people should be prepared to privilege this particular capacity.
I argue that the account of political equality with which deliberative democracy is bound up offers a compelling explanation
of this sort, even if it also gives rise to some difficult questions of feasibility. I conclude by considering the charge
that any political scheme that framed the relationship between deliberative democracy and the public interest in this way
would be undesirable. 相似文献
102.
Sophie von Stumm Sally Macintyre David G. Batty Heather Clark Ian J. Deary 《Intelligence》2010,38(1):202-211
In a birth cohort of 6281 men from Aberdeen, Scotland, social class of origin, childhood intelligence, childhood behavior disturbance and education were examined as predictors of status attainment in midlife (46 to 51 years). Social class of origin, intelligence and behavior disturbance were conceptualized as correlated predictors, whose effects were hypothesized to be partially mediated by educational qualifications. A structural equation model using Full Information Maximum Likelihood estimation confirmed that education had the strongest direct effect on status attainment at midlife. Furthermore, education partially mediated the effects of social class of origin and childhood intelligence, and fully mediated the effects of behavior disturbance on status attainment. Social class of origin, childhood intelligence and behavior disturbance were strongly inter-correlated. After controlling for these associations, educational and social status attainments were influenced to a considerably greater extent by childhood intelligence than by social class of origin. 相似文献
103.
104.
Julie Eyden Elizabeth J. Robinson Shiri Einav Vikram K. Jaswal 《Journal of experimental child psychology》2013
How do children evaluate the veracity of printed text? We examined children’s handling of unexpected suggestions conveyed via print versus orally. In Experiment 1 (N = 131), 3- to 6-year-olds witnessed a speaker either read aloud an unexpected but not completely implausible printed label (e.g., fish for a bird-like animal with some fish features) or speak the label without accompanying text. Pre-readers accepted labels in both conditions. Early readers often rejected spoken labels yet accepted them in the print condition, and in Experiment 2 (N = 55) 3- to 6-year-olds continued to apply them even after the print was obscured. Early readers accept printed testimony that they reject if only spoken, and the influence of text endures even when it is no longer visible. 相似文献
105.
Abstract Nigro and Neisser (1983) contrasted two ways of remembering personal experiences: the rememberer may ‘see’ the event from his or her perspective as in normal perception, or ‘see’ the self engaged in the event as an observer would. Several factors contribute to the determination of perspective, but Nigro and Neisser also reported that many subjects claimed they could change to another perspective at will. We sampled personal memories from several life periods and assessed ability to change the initially reported perspective. Changing was easier for recent or vividly recalled events, harder for older and less vividly recalled events. Memory perspectives may differ in other aspects than their imagery. A second study was conducted to determine whether affective experience is altered when perspectives are changed. The affect experienced decreased when shifting from a field to an observer perspective, but did not change with the converse shift. These studies provide further evidence that remembering is more than retrieval. The information that enters awareness is determined by the information sources in memory and the organisational scheme adopted for recollection. 相似文献
106.
107.
108.
Rhodes R Azzouni J Baumrin SB Benkov K Blaser MJ Brenner B Dauben JW Earle WJ Frank L Gligorov N Goldfarb J Hirschhorn K Hirschhorn R Holzman I Indyk D Jabs EW Lackey DP Moros DA Philpott S Rhodes ME Richardson LD Sacks HS Schwab A Sperling R Trusko B Zweig A 《The American journal of bioethics : AJOB》2011,11(11):1-7
109.
Erin K. Cressman Melanie Y. Lam Ian M. Franks James T. Enns Romeo Chua 《Consciousness and cognition》2013,22(3):716-728
We asked whether the influence of an invisible prime on movement is dependent on conscious movement expectations. Participants reached to a central target, which triggered a directional prime–mask arrow sequence. Participants were instructed that the visible arrows (masks) would most often signal a movement modification in a specific (biased) direction. Kinematic analyses revealed that responses to the visible mask were influenced by participants’ intentional bias, as movements were fastest when the more probable mask was displayed. In addition, responses were influenced by the invisible prime without regard to its relationship to the more probable mask. Analysis of the time of initial trajectory modifications revealed that both primes influenced responses in a similar manner after accounting for participants’ bias. These results imply that invisible stimuli automatically activate their associated responses and that unconscious priming of the motor system is insensitive to the conscious expectations of the participant making the pointing movements. 相似文献
110.
Athena Robinson 《Cognitive and behavioral practice》2013,20(1):93-105
Binge eating disorder (BED), a chronic condition characterized by eating disorder psychopathology and physical and social disability, represents a significant public health problem. Guided self-help (GSH) treatments for BED appear promising and may be more readily disseminable to mental health care providers, accessible to patients, and cost-effective than existing, efficacious BED specialty treatments, which are limited in public health utility and impact given their time and expense demands. No existing BED GSH treatment has incorporated affect regulation models of binge eating, which appears warranted given research linking negative affect and binge eating. This article describes Integrative Response Therapy (IRT), a new group-based guided self-help treatment based on the affect regulation model of binge eating, which has shown initial promise in a pilot sample of adults meeting DSM-IV criteria for BED. Fifty-four percent and 67% of participants were abstinent at posttreatment and 3-month follow-up, respectively. There was a significant reduction in the number of binge days over the previous 28 days from baseline to posttreatment [14.44 (± 7.16) to 3.15 (± 5.70); t = 7.71, p < .001; d = 2.2] and from baseline to follow-up [14.44 (± 7.16) to 1.50 (± 2.88); t = 5.64, p < .001; d = 1.7]. All subscales from both the Eating Disorder Examination–Questionnaire and Emotional Eating Scale were significantly lower at posttreatment compared to baseline. One hundred percent of IRT participants would recommend the program to a friend or family member in need. IRT's longer-term efficacy and acceptability are presently being tested in a NIMH-funded randomized controlled trial. 相似文献