全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1230篇 |
免费 | 25篇 |
专业分类
1255篇 |
出版年
2023年 | 12篇 |
2022年 | 12篇 |
2021年 | 9篇 |
2020年 | 14篇 |
2019年 | 25篇 |
2018年 | 37篇 |
2017年 | 28篇 |
2016年 | 35篇 |
2015年 | 22篇 |
2014年 | 22篇 |
2013年 | 147篇 |
2012年 | 35篇 |
2011年 | 54篇 |
2010年 | 31篇 |
2009年 | 27篇 |
2008年 | 35篇 |
2007年 | 46篇 |
2006年 | 32篇 |
2005年 | 36篇 |
2004年 | 33篇 |
2003年 | 41篇 |
2002年 | 33篇 |
2001年 | 33篇 |
2000年 | 26篇 |
1999年 | 32篇 |
1998年 | 17篇 |
1997年 | 12篇 |
1996年 | 12篇 |
1995年 | 8篇 |
1994年 | 10篇 |
1993年 | 12篇 |
1992年 | 16篇 |
1991年 | 22篇 |
1990年 | 17篇 |
1989年 | 24篇 |
1988年 | 13篇 |
1987年 | 16篇 |
1986年 | 18篇 |
1985年 | 13篇 |
1984年 | 13篇 |
1983年 | 14篇 |
1982年 | 15篇 |
1981年 | 15篇 |
1980年 | 16篇 |
1979年 | 9篇 |
1978年 | 8篇 |
1977年 | 14篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1974年 | 7篇 |
1970年 | 7篇 |
排序方式: 共有1255条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
71.
This study used three samples to investigate the nature of children's self-reported recess problems and the degree to which these were correlated with children's peer acceptance and mutual friendships. Results suggest that seven student complaints about recess can be grouped into problems with peer conflict and problems with social inclusion. Modest relations were reported between inclusion recess problems and children's mutual friendships and peer acceptance. Relations of conflict recess problems with mutual friendships and peer acceptance were less frequent. Results suggest that self-reported recess problems are a distinct construct from traditional research measures of peer acceptance and friendships. 相似文献
72.
Judith A. Hall Susan A. Andrzejewski Nora A. Murphy Marianne Schmid Mast Brian A. Feinstein 《Journal of research in personality》2008
Tests of accuracy in interpersonal perception take many forms. Often, such tests use designs and scoring methods that produce overall accuracy levels that cannot be directly compared across tests. Therefore, progress in understanding accuracy levels has been hampered. The present article employed several techniques for achieving score equivalency. Mean accuracy was converted to a common metric, pi [Rosenthal, R., & Rubin, D. B. (1989). Effect size estimation for one-sample multiple-choice-type data: Design, analysis, and meta-analysis. Psychological Bulletin, 106, 332–337] in a database of 109 published results representing tests that varied in terms of scoring method (proportion accuracy versus correlation), content (e.g., personality versus affect), number of response options, item preselection, cue channel (e.g., face versus voice), stimulus duration, and dynamism. Overall, accuracy was midway between guessing level and a perfect score, with accuracy being higher for tests based on preselected than unselected stimuli. When item preselection was held constant, accuracy was equivalent for judging affect and judging personality. However, comparisons must be made with caution due to methodological variations between studies and gaps in the literature. 相似文献
73.
Hunter A. McAllister 《Journal of applied social psychology》1990,20(18):1461-1473
The present experiment tested the relative impact of two types of eyewitness evidence (identification and non identification) on plea-bargain decisions by prosecutors and defense attorneys. A hypothetical case involving a robbery was mailed to three prosecutors and three defense attorneys in each of 47 states. The subjects were randomly assigned to receive a case in which an eyewitness claimed: (a) the defendant was the criminal (identification), (b) the defendant was not the criminal (non identification), or (c) it was not possible to tell whether the defendant was the criminal (control). Similar to findings with jurors, both prosecutors and defense attorneys underutilized the non identification information in making their plea-bargain decisions. In the case of the prosecutors, there was evidence that the underutilization of eyewitness non identification was at least partially mediated by the prosecutors' predictions of juror reaction to the evidence. 相似文献
74.
75.
76.
77.
78.
79.
80.