全文获取类型
收费全文 | 417篇 |
免费 | 19篇 |
专业分类
436篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 5篇 |
2021年 | 3篇 |
2020年 | 7篇 |
2019年 | 6篇 |
2018年 | 16篇 |
2017年 | 10篇 |
2016年 | 11篇 |
2015年 | 7篇 |
2014年 | 10篇 |
2013年 | 62篇 |
2012年 | 14篇 |
2011年 | 11篇 |
2010年 | 11篇 |
2009年 | 5篇 |
2008年 | 10篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 13篇 |
2005年 | 10篇 |
2004年 | 10篇 |
2003年 | 16篇 |
2002年 | 12篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 7篇 |
1999年 | 7篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1997年 | 4篇 |
1996年 | 5篇 |
1995年 | 3篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1993年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 5篇 |
1991年 | 7篇 |
1989年 | 5篇 |
1988年 | 6篇 |
1987年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 4篇 |
1984年 | 5篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1982年 | 8篇 |
1981年 | 7篇 |
1980年 | 10篇 |
1979年 | 5篇 |
1978年 | 6篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 6篇 |
1974年 | 3篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 3篇 |
1955年 | 3篇 |
排序方式: 共有436条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
71.
D. Stephen Lindsay Kimberley Wade Michael Hunter J. Don Read 《Memory (Hove, England)》2013,21(1):27-43
Adult questionnaire respondents reported, for each of a number of events, if they had experienced that event during childhood and, if so, if they remembered the experience or merely knew it had happened. Respondents also rated the emotion of each event and judged whether they would remember more about each reportedly experienced event if they spent more time trying to do so. Study 1 respondents were 96 undergraduates, whereas Study 2 tested 93 community members ranging widely in age. Respondents often reported no recollections of reportedly experienced events. Reportedly experienced events rated as emotional were more often recollected than those rated as neutral, and those rated as positive were more often recollected than those rated as negative. Predicted ability to remember more was related to current memory. Claims of remembering reportedly experienced events increased with age, but predicted ability to remember more about them declined with age. 相似文献
72.
Susan Vanderhill David F. Hultsch Michael A. Hunter Esther Strauss 《Neuropsychology, development, and cognition. Section B, Aging, neuropsychology and cognition》2013,20(4):385-405
ABSTRACT Insight into one's own cognitive abilities, or metacognition, has been widely studied in developmental psychology. Relevance to the clinician is high, as memory complaints in older adults show an association with impending dementia, even after controlling for likely confounds. Another candidate marker of impending dementia under study is inconsistency in cognitive performance over short time intervals. Although there has been a recent proliferation of studies of cognitive inconsistency in older adults, to date, no one has examined adults' self-perceptions of cognitive inconsistency. Ninety-four community-dwelling older adults (aged 70–91) were randomly selected from a parent longitudinal study of short-term inconsistency and long-term cognitive change in aging. Participants completed a novel 40-item self-report measure of everyday cognitive inconsistency, including parallel scales indexing perceived inconsistency 5 years ago and at present, yielding measures of past, present, and 5-year change in inconsistency. The questionnaire showed acceptable psychometric characteristics. The sample reported an increase in perceived inconsistency over time. Higher reported present inconsistency and greater 5-year increase in inconsistency were associated with noncognitive (e.g., older age, poorer ADLs, poorer health, higher depression), metacognitive (e.g., poorer self-rated memory) and neuropsychological (e.g., poorer performance and greater 5-year decline in global cognitive status, vocabulary, and memory) measures. Correlations between self-reported inconsistency and neuropsychological performance were attenuated, but largely persisted when self-rated memory and age were controlled. Observed relationships between self-reported inconsistency and measures of neuropsychological (including memory) status and decline suggest that self-perceived inconsistency may be an area of relevance in evaluating older adults for memory disorders. 相似文献
73.
The CaR-FA-X model (Williams et al., 2007), or capture and rumination (CaR), functional avoidance (FA), and impaired executive control (X), is a model of overgeneral autobiographical memory (OGM). Two mechanisms of the model, rumination and executive control, were examined in isolation and in interaction in order to investigate OGM over time. Across two time points, six months apart, a total of 149 adolescents (13–16 years) completed the minimal-instruction autobiographical memory test, a measure of executive control with both emotional and nonemotional stimuli, and measures of brooding rumination and reflective pondering. The results showed that executive control for emotional information was negatively associated with OGM, but only when reflective pondering levels were high. Therefore, in the context of higher levels of reflective pondering, greater switch costs (i.e., lower executive control) when processing emotional information predicted a decrease in OGM over time. 相似文献
74.
Wendy Hadley Amy Lansing David H. Barker Larry K. Brown Heather Hunter Geri Donenberg Ralph J. DiClemente 《Journal of child and family studies》2018,27(4):1098-1109
Parents can play a vital role in shaping teenagers’ sexual attitudes, behavior, and contraceptive use through communication, however, less is known about how to modify parent–adolescent communication among youth with mental health problems. The impact of a family-based sexual risk prevention intervention on both observational and self-report of parent–adolescent sexual communication was examined at 12 months among adolescents with mental health problems. Of the 721 parent–adolescent dyads recruited for the study, 167 videotapes of sexual discussions between parents and adolescent were coded for the family-based intervention and 191 videotapes for the active comparison. Longitudinal analyses examined differences between conditions (family-based vs. comparison) in self-reported and observed parent–adolescent sexual discussions and also examined the impact of gender on intervention response. More parent I-statements, healthier parent body language, and fewer adolescent Negative Vocalizations were detected for family-based intervention participants 12 months after participating in the brief intervention (11?h of total intervention time) relative to those in the comparison condition. Parents in the family-based intervention also self-reported better sexual communication at 12 months. The current study provides supporting evidence that a relatively brief family-based intervention was successful at addressing parent–adolescent sexual communication among a mental health sample. 相似文献
75.
Up to 80% of men who receive androgen deprivation therapy report hot flushes and for many these are associated with reduced quality of life. However it is recognised that there are a number of barriers to men’s engagement with support to manage symptoms and improve quality of life. This qualitative study was embedded within a larger randomised controlled trial (MANCAN) of a guided self-help cognitive behavioural intervention to manage hot flushes resulting among men receiving androgen deprivation therapy. The study aimed to explore the engagement and experiences with the guided self-help intervention. Twenty men recruited from the treatment arm of the MANCAN trial participated in a semi-structured interview exploring acceptability of the intervention, factors affecting engagement and perceived usefulness of the intervention. Interviews were audio-recorded, transcribed verbatim and analysed using a Framework approach. Over two thirds of respondents (69%) reported reading the intervention booklet in full and over 90% reporting practising the relaxation CD at least once a week. Analysis of the interviews identified three super-ordinate themes and these related to changes in hot flush symptomatology (learned to cope with hot flushes in new ways), the skills that participants had derived from the intervention (promoting relaxation and reducing stressors), and to a broader usefulness of the intervention (broader impact of the intervention and skills). The present study identified positive engagement with a guided self-help intervention and that men applied the skills developed through the intervention to help them undertake general lifestyle changes. Psycho-educational interventions (e.g. cognitive behaviour therapy, relaxation, and positive lifestyle elements) offer the potential to be both effective and well received by male cancer survivors. 相似文献
76.
The Availability of Psychological Services for Aged Care Residents in Australia: A Survey of Facility Staff 下载免费PDF全文
77.
Philosophia - I argue that while practical reasoning is essentially first personal it does not require having essentially first personal thoughts. I start with an example of good practical... 相似文献
78.
79.
Previous researchers have studied meditation practices as a means to understand consciousness as well as altered states of
consciousness. Various meditation techniques, such as Transcendental Meditation (TM) and Qigong, have been explored with source
localization tools; however, the concentrative meditation technique has yet to be fully studied in this manner. The current
study demonstrates findings, which outline differential activation in a self-referential default network during meditation
in participants who espouse themselves as regular concentrative meditation practitioners, as well as comparisons with a control
group practicing a modified version of the relaxation response. The results are compared with other putative experimental
findings employing other meditation techniques, and the findings outlined in the current study are discussed with respect
to changes in perceptual awareness often reported by meditators. 相似文献
80.
Carlson DS Grzywacz JG Ferguson M Hunter EM Clinch CR Arcury TA 《The Journal of applied psychology》2011,96(5):1045-1054
This study examined organizational levers that impact work-family experiences, participant health, and subsequent turnover. Using a sample of 179 women returning to full-time work 4 months after childbirth, we examined the associations of 3 job resources (job security, skill discretion, and schedule control) with work-to-family enrichment and the associations of 2 job demands (psychological requirements and nonstandard work schedules) with work-to-family conflict. Further, we considered subsequent impact of work-to-family conflict and enrichment on women's health (physical and mental health) 8 months after women returned to work and the impact of health on voluntary turnover 12 months after women returned to work. Having a nonstandard work schedule was directly and positively related to conflict, whereas schedule control buffered the effect of psychological requirements on conflict. Skill discretion and job security, both job resources, directly and positively related to enrichment. Work-to-family conflict was negatively related to both physical and mental health, but work-to-family enrichment positively predicted only physical health. Physical health and mental health both negatively influenced turnover. We discuss implications and opportunities for future research. 相似文献