全文获取类型
收费全文 | 664篇 |
免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
667篇 |
出版年
2024年 | 6篇 |
2021年 | 5篇 |
2020年 | 6篇 |
2019年 | 11篇 |
2018年 | 10篇 |
2017年 | 10篇 |
2016年 | 16篇 |
2015年 | 5篇 |
2014年 | 8篇 |
2013年 | 78篇 |
2012年 | 15篇 |
2011年 | 17篇 |
2010年 | 15篇 |
2009年 | 18篇 |
2008年 | 20篇 |
2007年 | 13篇 |
2006年 | 30篇 |
2005年 | 20篇 |
2004年 | 24篇 |
2003年 | 24篇 |
2002年 | 18篇 |
2001年 | 13篇 |
2000年 | 18篇 |
1999年 | 9篇 |
1998年 | 8篇 |
1997年 | 7篇 |
1996年 | 9篇 |
1995年 | 11篇 |
1994年 | 9篇 |
1993年 | 10篇 |
1992年 | 9篇 |
1991年 | 8篇 |
1990年 | 13篇 |
1989年 | 10篇 |
1988年 | 11篇 |
1987年 | 13篇 |
1986年 | 6篇 |
1985年 | 5篇 |
1984年 | 9篇 |
1983年 | 7篇 |
1981年 | 6篇 |
1980年 | 9篇 |
1979年 | 6篇 |
1978年 | 11篇 |
1977年 | 8篇 |
1976年 | 9篇 |
1975年 | 5篇 |
1974年 | 8篇 |
1971年 | 9篇 |
1968年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有667条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
191.
Hester R Barre N Mattingley JB Foxe JJ Garavan H 《Cognitive, affective & behavioral neuroscience》2007,7(4):317-326
The magnitude of posterior medial frontal cortex (pMFC) activity during commission of an error has been shown to relate to adaptive posterror changes in response behavior on the trial immediately following. In the present article, we examined neural activity during and after error commission to identify its relationship to sustained posterror behavior changes that led to performance improvements several trials into the future. The standard task required participants to inhibit a prepotent motor response during infrequent lure trials, which were randomly interspersed among numerous go trials. Posterror behavior was manipulated by introducing a dynamic condition, in which an error on a lure trial ensured that the next lure would appear within two to seven go trials. Behavioral data indicated significantly higher levels of posterror slowing and accuracy during the dynamic condition, as well as fewer consecutive lure errors. Bilateral prefrontal cortex (PFC) and pMFC activity during the posterror period, but not during commission of the error itself, was associated with increased posterror slowing. Activity within two of these regions (right PFC and pMFC) also predicted success on the next lure trial. The findings support a relationship between pMFC/PFC activity and adaptive posterror behavior change, and the discrepancy between these findings and those of previous studies-in the present study, this relationship was detected during the posterror period rather than during commission of the error itself--may have resulted from the requirements of the present task. Implications of this discrepancy for the flexibility of cognitive control are discussed. 相似文献
192.
Allan Hugh Cole Jr. 《Pastoral Psychology》2009,58(5-6):531-549
This essay posits a relationship between male melancholia, defined as a complicated form of grief that resists compensatory consolation, and experiences of identity-loss. I draw especially from pastoral psychologist Donald Capps’s views on male melancholia. Concurring with Capps’s claim that melancholic men seek relief from melancholia in religion, I also draw from the psychology of Erik H. Erikson to add this emphasis: that both the roots of melancholia and subsequent experiences of melancholic loss link with experiences of perceived threats to or loss of one’s identity. I also suggest how religious rituals may ameliorate melancholia. 相似文献
193.
Alan Kennedy Thierry Baccino 《The Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology Section A: Human Experimental Psychology》1995,48(1):55-71
Two experiments are described in which subjects attempted to locate a specified target word in a short text using a cursor controlled by a computer mouse pointing device. The task was performed at screen refresh rates of 50 Hz, 75 Hz, and 100 Hz. In Experiment 1, both the timing and accuracy of the cursor movement was influenced by screen pulsation. During the early phase of the movement, performance was worse at 100 Hz, whereas in the later, visually guided phase, performance was worse at 50 Hz. In Experiment 2, eye movements were recorded as the task was performed. The results show that the cursor movement is typically preceded by an eye movement and that subjects do not directly inspect the cursor in the early stages of its movement. In the later phase of the movement the cursor is tracked for considerable periods of time. The data suggest that adverse effects of screen pulsation on the control of cursor movement are inherited from penalties incurred during the process of target computation but may also be influenced by concurrent eye movements. 相似文献
194.
Thomas G. Haring Ph.D. Catherine G. Breen M.A. Jan Weiner Ph.D. Craig H. Kennedy Ph.D. Florene Bednersh Ph.D. 《Journal of Behavioral Education》1995,5(1):29-53
Six students with moderate and severe disabilities were taught generalized purchasing skills through the use of videotape modeling in one, two, or three stores and in vivo instruction within one store. Training was conducted within three orders of treatments: (a) in vivo instruction followed by videotape training, (b) videotape training followed by in vivo instruction, and (c) concurrent videotape and in vivo instruction. Generalization probes were conducted in stores that were the same as those modeled on videotape, untrained stores that were infrequently probed (and never modeled on tape or taught directly), and novel stores that were probed only once after training. Results indicated the production of generalized purchasing skills by the students who received concurrent training and by the students who received sequential training. Videotape and in vivo training in isolation did not lead to generalized shopping skills. The results are discussed in terms of investigating the possible role of verbalization during videotape training on generalized responding and the effects of multiple probe interventions on inadvertent learning of critical skills.deceased. 相似文献
195.
In order to assess the hypotheses that Alzheimer′s disease (AD) results in a property level restructuring, loss, or degradation of lexical-semantic knowledge, Alzheimer′s patients and normal elderly subjects were presented with a property verification task in which they were asked to judge the truth value of telegraphic statements which paired objects with their properties (e.g., "Apple is red"). Objects with either high- or low-typical exemplars of categories (e.g., "oak" is a high typical exemplar of the category "tree," while "palm" is a less typical item). Properties were varied with respect to normatively determined dominance (e.g, "fins" is a high dominant property of "trout," while "slimy" is a less dominant property) and whether they were distinctive (i.e., served to distinguish between subsets of exemplars within a category) or shared among most or all category members (e.g., "stem" for the category "fruit"). Analyses of accuracy and reaction time data suggested that AD results in neither a loss per se of representation of properties, nor a reorganization of relations between objects′ properties. However, results were consistent with a property level degradation of AD patients′ object concepts. While there was no evidence for a differential degradation of distinctive vs shared properties, results suggested that AD patients have degraded representations of lower dominant properties and properties of low-typical category exemplars. 相似文献
196.
Strong empirical evidence was obtained in support of the proposition that nonabsolute/relativistic (N/R) thinking is a unifying commonality underlying four postformal models of higher-order thinking, namely, problem finding, dialectical reasoning, relativistic operations, and reflective judgment. Within the construct of N/R thinking, two dimensions—the basic form and the epistemic view—and two levels—the formal form and the postformal form—were differentiated as hypothesized. The findings clarify logical relationships among the models as well as provide a common link unifying their diversity. Implications and suggestions for future research are provided. 相似文献
197.
198.
199.
200.