全文获取类型
收费全文 | 2519篇 |
免费 | 1371篇 |
国内免费 | 14篇 |
专业分类
3904篇 |
出版年
2013年 | 116篇 |
2012年 | 21篇 |
2011年 | 66篇 |
2010年 | 108篇 |
2009年 | 178篇 |
2008年 | 243篇 |
2007年 | 86篇 |
2006年 | 44篇 |
2005年 | 152篇 |
2004年 | 166篇 |
2003年 | 81篇 |
2002年 | 48篇 |
2001年 | 137篇 |
2000年 | 43篇 |
1999年 | 94篇 |
1998年 | 83篇 |
1997年 | 50篇 |
1996年 | 89篇 |
1995年 | 69篇 |
1994年 | 74篇 |
1993年 | 110篇 |
1992年 | 132篇 |
1991年 | 122篇 |
1990年 | 115篇 |
1989年 | 122篇 |
1988年 | 99篇 |
1987年 | 85篇 |
1986年 | 79篇 |
1985年 | 33篇 |
1984年 | 71篇 |
1982年 | 74篇 |
1981年 | 31篇 |
1980年 | 48篇 |
1979年 | 38篇 |
1976年 | 20篇 |
1974年 | 19篇 |
1973年 | 32篇 |
1972年 | 45篇 |
1971年 | 46篇 |
1970年 | 29篇 |
1969年 | 32篇 |
1968年 | 56篇 |
1967年 | 38篇 |
1966年 | 48篇 |
1965年 | 40篇 |
1964年 | 80篇 |
1963年 | 73篇 |
1962年 | 48篇 |
1961年 | 38篇 |
1959年 | 22篇 |
排序方式: 共有3904条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
51.
Choice between delayed reinforcers and fixed-ratio schedules requiring forceful responding. 总被引:1,自引:1,他引:0
下载免费PDF全文

This experiment measured pigeons' choices between delayed reinforcers and fixed-ratio schedules in which a force of approximately 0.48 N was needed to operate the response key. In ratio-delay conditions, subjects chose between a fixed-ratio schedule and an adjusting delay. The delay was increased or decreased several times a session in order to estimate an indifference point--a delay duration at which the two alternatives were chosen about equally often. Each ratio-delay condition was followed by a delay-delay condition in which subjects chose between the adjusting delay and a variable-time schedule, with the components of this schedule selected to match the ratio completion times of the preceding ratio-delay condition. The adjusting delays at the indifference point were longer when the alternative was a fixed-ratio schedule than when it was a matched variable-time schedule, which indicated a preference for the matched variable-time schedules over the fixed-ratio schedules. This preference increased in a nonlinear manner with increasing ratio size. This nonlinearity was inconsistent with a theory that states that indifference points for both time and ratio schedules can be predicted by multiplying the choice response-reinforcer intervals of the two types of schedules by different multiplicative constants. Two other theories, which predict nonlinear increases in preference for the matched variable-time schedules, are discussed. 相似文献
52.
53.
This experiment compared the extent to which students learned facts included in computer-based-training frames that required
an overt response to those that did not. Frames included two types of facts: Some facts had one word missing which had to
be supplied by the student (active facts). Other facts had no missing words and required no overt response (passive facts).
Each student completed four 14-fact modules in random order. Two of the modules contained frames with all active facts. Each
frame in the other two modules contained one active and six passive facts. Paper pretests and posttests showed that students
learned twice as many facts when all facts in the module were active than when only one in seven were active. The modules
that included passive facts required one-fifth the time to complete than those with only active facts. Students learned more
facts per minute of training in one of the two modules that included passive facts than in the two modules with only active
facts. 相似文献
54.
55.
Richard L. Port Heather A. Murphy R. DAniel Miller Kevin S. Seybold 《Current Psychology》1997,15(4):332-339
present research examined the role of hippocampal NMDA-dependent synaptic potentiation on appetitive instrumental conditioning
under a continuous reinforcement schedule. In the first experiment, low (.025 mg.kg) or moderate (.05 mg/kg) dosages of the
NMDA receptor antagonist, MK801, failed to increase the number of training days required to reach acquisition criterion; number
of training days required to reach criterion for extinction were also unaffected. In the second experiment, a higher dosage
(.10 mg/kg) of MK801 or induction of long-term potentiation failed to alter the number of responses occurring during acquisition.
These data suggest that hippocampal synaptic potentiation does not play a prominent role in instrumental learning with simple
contingency conditions. It is suggested that hippocampal LTP reflects a perceptual process that contributes differentially
to spatial cognition, classical and instrumental conditioning. 相似文献
56.
57.
Food-deprived rats (at 80% of their free-feeding weights) were exposed to a fixed-time 60-s schedule of food-pellet presentation and developed schedule-induced drinking. Lick-dependent signaled delays (10 s) to food presentation led to decreased drinking, which recovered when the signaled delays were discontinued. A major effect of this punishment contingency was to increase the proportion of interpellet intervals without any licks. The drinking of yoked control rats, which received food at the same times as those exposed to the signaled delay contingency (masters), was not consistently reduced. When food-deprivation level was changed to 90%, all master and yoked control rats showed decreases in punished or unpunished schedule-induced drinking. When the body weights were reduced to 70%, most master rats increased punished behavior to levels similar to those of unpunished drinking. This effect was not observed for yoked controls. Therefore, body-weight loss increased the resistance of schedule-induced drinking to reductions by punishment. Food-deprivation effects on punished schedule-induced drinking are similar to their effects on food-maintained lever pressing. This dependency of punishment on food-deprivation level supports the view that schedule-induced drinking can be modified by the same variables that affect operant behavior in general. 相似文献
58.
59.
60.