全文获取类型
收费全文 | 262篇 |
免费 | 11篇 |
专业分类
273篇 |
出版年
2022年 | 2篇 |
2020年 | 2篇 |
2019年 | 7篇 |
2018年 | 7篇 |
2017年 | 6篇 |
2016年 | 8篇 |
2015年 | 3篇 |
2014年 | 2篇 |
2013年 | 21篇 |
2012年 | 10篇 |
2011年 | 12篇 |
2010年 | 9篇 |
2008年 | 7篇 |
2007年 | 10篇 |
2006年 | 10篇 |
2005年 | 7篇 |
2004年 | 8篇 |
2003年 | 12篇 |
2002年 | 8篇 |
2001年 | 10篇 |
2000年 | 5篇 |
1999年 | 11篇 |
1998年 | 6篇 |
1996年 | 2篇 |
1994年 | 3篇 |
1992年 | 6篇 |
1991年 | 3篇 |
1990年 | 4篇 |
1989年 | 4篇 |
1988年 | 5篇 |
1986年 | 3篇 |
1984年 | 4篇 |
1982年 | 3篇 |
1980年 | 2篇 |
1978年 | 2篇 |
1976年 | 2篇 |
1971年 | 2篇 |
1968年 | 4篇 |
1967年 | 2篇 |
1966年 | 2篇 |
1965年 | 1篇 |
1964年 | 2篇 |
1963年 | 1篇 |
1961年 | 3篇 |
1960年 | 3篇 |
1959年 | 1篇 |
1956年 | 1篇 |
1953年 | 1篇 |
1952年 | 2篇 |
1950年 | 5篇 |
排序方式: 共有273条查询结果,搜索用时 31 毫秒
41.
Observers are poor at reporting the identities of objects that they have successfully tracked (Pylyshyn, Visual Cognition, 11, 801-822, 2004; Scholl & Pylyshyn, Cognitive Psychology, 38, 259-290, 1999). Consequently, it has been claimed that objects are tracked in a manner that does not encode their identities (Pylyshyn, 2004). Here, we present evidence that disputes this claim. In a series of experiments, we show that attempting to track the identities of objects can decrease an observer's ability to track the objects' locations. This indicates that the mechanisms that track, respectively, the locations and identities of objects draw upon a common resource. Furthermore, we show that this common resource can be voluntarily distributed between the two mechanisms. This is clear evidence that the location- and identity-tracking mechanisms are not entirely dissociable. 相似文献
42.
Howe ML 《Psychological bulletin》2008,134(5):768-72; discussion 773-7
In this commentary, assumptions about the nature and development of children's false memories as described in a recent article by C. J. Brainerd, V. F. Reyna, and S. J. Ceci (2008) are reviewed. Specifically, questions are raised about what drives the development of false memories in fuzzy-trace theory (FTT). Recent studies that challenge a core assumption of FTT, that false memory illusions increase across development as children learn to establish meaningful connections across items (i.e., establish gist), are discussed. An alternative conceptualization of the development of false memory illusions, associative-activation theory (AAT), is presented. AAT provides as viable an account of the development of false memory illusions as does FTT and anticipates a unique set of outcomes that have recently appeared in the developmental literature on false memory illusions. 相似文献
43.
Children's emotional false memories 总被引:1,自引:0,他引:1
Howe ML 《Psychological science》2007,18(10):856-860
Eight- and 12-year-old children were presented with neutral and negative emotional Deese-Roediger-McDermott lists equated on familiarity and associative strength. Both recall and recognition (A') measures were obtained. Recall measures exhibited the usual age increments in true and false recollection. True neutral items were better recalled and recognized than true negative emotional items. Although the children showed more false recall for neutral than for negative emotional lists, false recognition was higher for negative emotional than for neutral items. A' analyses also showed that whereas true neutral information and false neutral information were easily discriminated by children regardless of age, the same was not the case for true and false negative emotional information. Together, these results suggest that although children may be able to censor negative emotional information at recall, such information promotes relational processing in children's memory, making true and false emotional information less discriminable overall. 相似文献
44.
45.
C.J. Brainerd M.L. Howe J. Kingma S.H. Brainerd 《Journal of experimental child psychology》1984,37(3):478-499
We report two free recall experiments and a cued recall experiment in which a new two-stage model was used to obtain numerical measurements of age changes in various aspects of storage and retrieval. The subjects in all three experiments were 7-year-olds (second graders) and 11-year-olds (sixth graders). The major findings in the free recall experiment were that getting a trace into storage posed less of a problem for elementary schoolers than learning how to get it out on test trials, that retrieval development is more rapid during the elementary school years than storage development, and that the superiority of older children's storage and retrieval abilities tends to become more pronounced as learning progresses. A similar pattern of results was obtained under different conditions in the cued recall experiment. 相似文献
46.
Leslie A. Howe 《Journal of applied philosophy》2020,37(5):764-780
I argue that athletes should neither be taken as role models nor present themselves as such. Indeed, they should resist any attempt to take them as such on the grounds that seeing athletes (or other celebrities) as role models abrogates the existential and ethical responsibilities of both parties. Whether one takes on the role of being a model to others or whether one chooses to model one’s own behaviour on that of another, except in respect of the development of technical skill, one engages in bad faith and fails to act in an existentially ethical way. To this end, I consider the inclination to influence and admiration, as well as self-respect, agency, motivation, and envy. I argue that while celebrity sportspersons and similar others do not have duties that others do not have just by virtue of their notoriety alone, they may have situational responsibilities commensurate with their social reach. More importantly, however, since social influence is not a transformation of inert matter, there are also responsibilities belonging to their audiences, those who merely observe and those who choose to imitate 相似文献
47.
48.
Michael J. A. Howe 《Applied cognitive psychology》1990,4(2):145-146
49.
50.