全文获取类型
收费全文 | 59089篇 |
免费 | 1036篇 |
国内免费 | 3篇 |
专业分类
60128篇 |
出版年
2020年 | 701篇 |
2019年 | 823篇 |
2018年 | 1153篇 |
2017年 | 1183篇 |
2016年 | 1236篇 |
2015年 | 896篇 |
2014年 | 1067篇 |
2013年 | 5058篇 |
2012年 | 1970篇 |
2011年 | 1985篇 |
2010年 | 1193篇 |
2009年 | 1227篇 |
2008年 | 1702篇 |
2007年 | 1685篇 |
2006年 | 1540篇 |
2005年 | 1277篇 |
2004年 | 1317篇 |
2003年 | 1272篇 |
2002年 | 1206篇 |
2001年 | 1953篇 |
2000年 | 1819篇 |
1999年 | 1360篇 |
1998年 | 640篇 |
1997年 | 551篇 |
1996年 | 625篇 |
1995年 | 569篇 |
1994年 | 556篇 |
1993年 | 544篇 |
1992年 | 1124篇 |
1991年 | 1048篇 |
1990年 | 1007篇 |
1989年 | 974篇 |
1988年 | 948篇 |
1987年 | 883篇 |
1986年 | 879篇 |
1985年 | 929篇 |
1984年 | 758篇 |
1983年 | 655篇 |
1982年 | 512篇 |
1979年 | 781篇 |
1978年 | 586篇 |
1975年 | 626篇 |
1974年 | 711篇 |
1973年 | 722篇 |
1972年 | 618篇 |
1971年 | 581篇 |
1970年 | 500篇 |
1968年 | 619篇 |
1967年 | 541篇 |
1966年 | 517篇 |
排序方式: 共有10000条查询结果,搜索用时 0 毫秒
931.
Alan S. Bellack Samuel M. Turner Michel Hersen Raymond Luber 《Journal of psychopathology and behavioral assessment》1980,2(2):99-104
The purpose of this study was to examine the role of stress in performance on role play tests of social skill. Chronic psychiatric patients were tested in a formal videotape studio or in an informal consulting room setting. Motoric and physiological measures were secured. In addition, one-half of the subjects were retested one week later. In contrast to previous studies, results indicated that the assessment was not stressful. The two groups were not different on any measure, and the mean heart rates ranged from only 80 to 84 b.p.m. The effects of retesting were difficult to interpret. There were reductions in motoric indices of anxiety (speech disruptions) but not on physiological indices. Overall, testretest reliability was quite high. The results raise several questions which affect interpretation of many social skills studies in the literature. 相似文献
932.
933.
Ronald S. Harwerth Earl L. Smith Dennis M. Levi 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1980,27(1):43-50
Binocular interactions of suprathreshold grating patterns have been investigated using a reaction time measure of contrast detection. Simple reaction times were determined for monocular and binocular viewing conditions over a contrast range from .63 to near threshold. The results from all subjects showed binocular summation for contrast levels near threshold, but there was considerable variation across subjects for contrast levels above threshold. Some subjects showed summation over the entire contrast range, but other subjects showed either binocular inhibition or binocular facilitation for some range of contrast levels. The pattern of binocular interaction for a given subject was consistent for several spatial frequencies. The differences in types of interaction between subjects, the variation in magnitude of binocular interaction with contrast level for each subject, and the data from experiments involving stimulation of noncorresponding retinal areas show that the binocular interactions found for suprathreshold stimuli cannot be accounted for on the basis of probability, and must, therefore, result from physiological interactions between the two eyes. These interactions have been investigated further under conditions of (1) induced fixation disparity, (2) horizontal gratings, and (3) orthogonally oriented gratings. 相似文献
934.
Gregory R. Lockhead Robert C. Johnson Fran M. Gold 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1980,27(6):545-549
At least three localization illusions can be produced by varying the time relations between two stimuli. Two of these, phi motion and Békésy’s illusion, have been induced in areas lacking receptors. To learn if this also occurred with the third illusion, sensory saltation, people reported their observations when Geldard’s visual rabbit was induced by stimulating points around the optic disk. The rabbit crossed the blind spot as well as other portions of the eyes, and the illusory point was often localized within the region of the blind spot. All three illusions can be localized in an area devoid of receptors. 相似文献
935.
Robert M. Gleeson Michael G. Dragunow Neil F. Kirton John W. Villiger Douglas L. Chute 《Behavior research methods》1980,12(3):346-348
Special difficulties are encountered when attempting to provide intracranial cannulation of small animals that have floating or fragile skull bones. The technique described was developed for mice but seems applicable to other species. The cannula system is inexpensive and easy to use. A wound clip anchor seems superior to either skull screws or bonded acrylic, producing less damage to the brain and no necrosis of bone or tissue on the wound margin. Patency data up to 25 days is presented, and the system is as efficacious as other techniques. 相似文献
936.
Two experiments were conducted to examine the role of sensory persistence on tasks of perceived duration employing very brief visual stimuli. Using a standard temporal judgment task, the first experiment replicated both the “size effect” and “empty-filled” illusion reported by previous investigators. However, entirely comparable results were also found with a probematching task, which theoretically assesses the degree of persistence exhibited by a stimulus. The second experiment examined the effect of target luminance on perceived duration. Consistent with a sensory persistence interpretation, judgments of duration increased with increasing luminance. The results from the two experiments were discussed in terms of varying degrees of retinal persistence produced by different stimuli. This view was contrasted with currently dominant interpretations that postulate changes in perceived duration to reflect different information-processing requirements across stimulus conditions. 相似文献
937.
Two experiments are reported that attempt to demonstrate a critical role played by sensory persistence on a standard perceived-duration task employing brief visual stimuli. Experiment 1 examined the effect on perceived duration of varying the spatial frequency of a target. For both 40- and 70-msec flashes, increased spatial frequency resulted in reduced estimates of perceived duration. These results were contrasted with predictions derived from cognitive processing models of duration perception. In Experiment 2, three typical types of target employed in current research (an outlined circle, a “noise”-filled circle, and a completely filled circle) were shown to differ significantly in their perceived duration and in their sensitivity to increases in physical duration. The results were discussed in terms of variable degrees of retinal persistence produced by the three types of targets. The possible implications for specific discrepancies in the literature and across-study comparisons in general were enumerated. 相似文献
938.
Patterns varying in form goodness and size were presented for 15, 30, and 45 msec. The experiment included three independent groups of subjects. Half of the first group were presented size variations with good figures, while the other half were shown size variations with poor figures. Half of the second group were presented goodness variations with small figures, while the other half were shown goodness variations with large figures. A third group experienced variations in both goodness and size. The major findings were: (1) poor figures and large figures were judged “longer” than good or small figures when varied within a session; (2) this effect vanished when the different levels of size or goodness were presented to separate groups of subjects; (3) the magnitude of the difference in temporal estimation was the same for size and goodness when each was varied in isolation; (4) when size and form goodness were varied orthogonally in the same session, both dimensions produced reliable changes in temporal estimation. These data are discussed in light of current theoretical explanations of the filled duration illusion. 相似文献
939.
James R. Sawusch Howard C. Nusbaum Eileen C. Schwab 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1980,27(5):421-434
Recent experiments have indicated that contrast effects can be obtained with vowels by anchoring a test series with one of the endpoint vowels. These contextual effects cannot be attributed to feature detector fatigue or to the induction of an overt response bias. In the present studies, anchored ABX discrimination functions and signal detection analyses of identification data (before and after anchoring) for an [i]-[I] vowel series were used to demonstrate that [i] and [I] anchoring produce contrast effects by affecting different perceptual mechanisms. The effects of [i] anchoring were to increase within-[i] category sensitivity, while [I] anchoring shifted criterion placements. When vowels were placed in CVC syllables to reduce available auditory memory, there was a significant decrease in the size of the [I]-anchor contrast effects. The magnitude of the [i]-anchor effect was unaffected by the reduction in vowel information available in auditory memory. These results suggest that [i] and [I] anchors affect mechanisms at different levels of processing. The [i] anchoring results may reflect normalization processes in speech perception that operate at an early level of perceptual processing, while the [I] anchoring results represent changes in response criterion mediated by auditory memory for vowel information. 相似文献
940.
D. M. Parker 《Attention, perception & psychophysics》1980,28(4):365-368
Response latencies to the onset, offset, and contrast reversal of sinusoidal gratings over a range of spatial frequencies were measured. For gratings of constant physical contrast, RT was monotonically related to spatial frequency regardless of presentation mode. Comparison of RTs to 1.0- and 9.0-cycle/deg gratings adjusted to equal apparent contrast showed that the RT shifts cannot be directly attributed to contrast sensitivity differences. It is concluded that spatial-frequency-dependent processing delays occur regardless of which temporal property of the stimulus the subject must respond to. 相似文献