全文获取类型
收费全文 | 1757篇 |
免费 | 32篇 |
专业分类
1789篇 |
出版年
2021年 | 14篇 |
2020年 | 21篇 |
2019年 | 17篇 |
2018年 | 27篇 |
2017年 | 45篇 |
2016年 | 33篇 |
2015年 | 34篇 |
2014年 | 26篇 |
2013年 | 238篇 |
2012年 | 46篇 |
2011年 | 56篇 |
2010年 | 34篇 |
2009年 | 36篇 |
2008年 | 46篇 |
2007年 | 60篇 |
2006年 | 56篇 |
2005年 | 46篇 |
2004年 | 37篇 |
2003年 | 46篇 |
2002年 | 53篇 |
2001年 | 25篇 |
2000年 | 22篇 |
1999年 | 21篇 |
1998年 | 21篇 |
1997年 | 16篇 |
1996年 | 20篇 |
1995年 | 15篇 |
1994年 | 20篇 |
1993年 | 16篇 |
1992年 | 29篇 |
1991年 | 30篇 |
1990年 | 14篇 |
1988年 | 15篇 |
1987年 | 19篇 |
1986年 | 26篇 |
1985年 | 26篇 |
1984年 | 29篇 |
1983年 | 27篇 |
1982年 | 34篇 |
1981年 | 22篇 |
1980年 | 28篇 |
1979年 | 24篇 |
1978年 | 25篇 |
1977年 | 24篇 |
1976年 | 23篇 |
1975年 | 19篇 |
1974年 | 31篇 |
1973年 | 23篇 |
1971年 | 16篇 |
1970年 | 18篇 |
排序方式: 共有1789条查询结果,搜索用时 15 毫秒
261.
262.
Keith Bredemeier Howard Berenbaum Steven B. Most Daniel J. Simons 《Cognition & emotion》2013,27(8):1510-1519
Anxiety and depression are often associated with attention control deficits, but few studies have explored whether neuroticism can account for these links. In the present study, undergraduate students (n=146) completed self-report measures of neuroticism, worry, anxious arousal, and anhedonic depression and also completed a visual attention task in which they were asked to identify a red target letter embedded within a rapid sequence of items. Neuroticism was associated with detection of the target when it was preceded by a distracter with which it shared a feature in common (a green letter). Specifically, these distracters produced longer attentional blinks in individuals with elevated levels of neuroticism. In contrast, target detection was not significantly associated with worry, anxious arousal, or anhedonic depression. We discuss the implications of this link between neuroticism and attention for cognitive models of emotional distress and disorders. 相似文献
263.
Howard B. Levine M.D. 《Psychoanalytic Inquiry》2014,34(3):214-224
This article articulates some of the problems that surround the use of the term trauma in psychoanalytic theory and suggests that the key element for a theory of pathogenesis and mental functioning is not the either/or of external versus internal causation or trauma versus drive. Rather, it is an understanding of whether, or to what extent, the raw data of existential experience is or is not transformed into psychological experience. From this perspective, trauma is whatever outstrips and disrupts the psyche’s capacity for representation or mentalization. Absent the potential for mental representation, these events and phenomena are historical only from an external, third-person perspective. Until they are mentalized, they remain locked within an ahistorical, repetitive process as potentials for action, somatization, and projection. 相似文献
264.
Howard E. Gruber 《创造力研究杂志》2013,25(1):27-51
This study investigated the connection between drawing skill and artistic creativity in relation to age groups and levels of artistic involvement among 223 Chinese primary, secondary, and university students in Hong Kong. Based on the fantasy drawing task, visual-artist judges made expert judgments on students' drawing skill and artistic creativity. There was substantial correlation between drawing skill and artistic creativity among children, adolescents, and young adults, even when artistic involvement was controlled in the partial correlation analyses. The results of correlational and regression analyses suggested that drawing skill contributed most significantly to the prediction of artistic creativity throughout the years at different stages of development, but artistic involvement became more important, especially in young adulthood. Implications of the findings on the skill–creativity relationship for arts education and the directions for future research are discussed. 相似文献
265.
266.
267.
268.
Bacon and Egeth (1994) proposed that observed instances of attentional capture by feature singletons (e.g., color) were the
result of a salience-based strategy adopted by subjects (singleton detection mode) and, thus, were not automatic. They showed that subjects could override capture by adopting strategies based on searching
for specific target features (feature search mode). However, Theeuwes (2004) has recently argued that Bacon and Egeth’s results arose from experimental confounds. He elaborated
a model in which attentional capture must be expected when salient distractors fall within a spatialwindow of attention. According to Theeuwes’s (2004) model, there exist two essential criteria for examining stimulus-driven capture.
First, search latencies cannot increase with display size, since the search must be parallel; second, the salience of the
irrelevant distractor must not be compromised by characteristics of the search display. Contrary to the predictions of Theeuwes’s
(2004) model, we provide evidence that involuntary capture can be overridden when both of these criteria are met. Our results
are consistent with Bacon and Egeth’s proposal. 相似文献
269.
Baron-Cohen S Ring H Chitnis X Wheelwright S Gregory L Williams S Brammer M Bullmore E 《Brain and cognition》2006,61(1):122-130
BACKGROUND: People with autism or Asperger Syndrome (AS) show altered patterns of brain activity during visual search and emotion recognition tasks. Autism and AS are genetic conditions and parents may show the 'broader autism phenotype.' AIMS: (1) To test if parents of children with AS show atypical brain activity during a visual search and an empathy task; (2) to test for sex differences during these tasks at the neural level; (3) to test if parents of children with autism are hyper-masculinized, as might be predicted by the 'extreme male brain' theory. METHOD: We used fMRI during a visual search task (the Embedded Figures Test (EFT)) and an emotion recognition test (the 'Reading the Mind in the Eyes' (or Eyes) test). SAMPLE: Twelve parents of children with AS, vs. 12 sex-matched controls. DESIGN: Factorial analysis was used to map main effects of sex, group (parents vs. controls), and sexxgroup interaction on brain function. An ordinal ANOVA also tested for regions of brain activity where females>males>fathers=mothers, to test for parental hyper-masculinization. RESULTS ON EFT TASK: Female controls showed more activity in extrastriate cortex than male controls, and both mothers and fathers showed even less activity in this area than sex-matched controls. There were no differences in group activation between mothers and fathers of children with AS. The ordinal ANOVA identified two specific regions in visual cortex (right and left, respectively) that showed the pattern Females>Males>Fathers=Mothers, both in BA 19. RESULTS ON EYES TASK: Male controls showed more activity in the left inferior frontal gyrus than female controls, and both mothers and fathers showed even more activity in this area compared to sex-matched controls. Female controls showed greater bilateral inferior frontal activation than males. This was not seen when comparing mothers to males, or mothers to fathers. The ordinal ANOVA identified two specific regions that showed the pattern Females>Males>Mothers=Fathers: left medial temporal gyrus (BA 21) and left dorsolateral prefrontal cortex (BA 44). CONCLUSIONS: Parents of children with AS show atypical brain function during both visual search and emotion recognition, in the direction of hyper-masculinization of the brain. Because of the small sample size, and lack of age-matching between parents and controls, such results constitute a pilot study that needs replicating with larger samples. 相似文献
270.
Studies of the relationship between stimulus-response uncertainty and reaction times indicate three qualitatively different functions: Hick's law, simple-choice step function, or flat curve (no effect at all). The extent of stimulus-response S-R) compatibility appears to interact with the effects of uncertainty on response times. One possible hypothesis regarding these various S-R uncertainty functions is that uncertainty will have an effect whenever the stimuli and their associated responses are not within the same egocentric spatial coordinates. We tested this hypothesis in 5 undergraduate participants (2 men, M age 18.7 yr., range 18-20) by investigating the time-course of pointing to peripherally located visual targets under four different levels of uncertainty (1, 2, 4, or 8 possible locations). Surprisingly, the resulting response function does not match any of those previously reported. Visually guided pointing produced a quadratic reaction time function as S-R uncertainty increases in log2 steps from 1 to 8. 相似文献